ГЛАВА 17 «ДО И ПОСЛЕ РОЖДЕНИЯ»



1 (Kane, 2006b); epigraph and block quotation included.

2 (Summers, 2005), para. 5.

3 Quoted in (Edge, 2005b).

4 (Brizendine, 2007), p. 34.

5 (Sax, 2006), p. 28.

6 (Kane, 2009), p. 373.

7 (Rothman, 1988), p. 130.

8 (Smith, 2005), pp. 51 and 52, respectively.

9 (Nosek, 2007b), p. 184.

10 See (Greenwald et al., 2009).

11 (Gonzalez & Koestner, 2005), p. 407.

12 (Jost, Pelham, & Carvallo, 2002), p. 597.

13 Jost et al. found even stronger evidence of implicit paternalism in the nontraditional sample (in which the mother’s last name was not the same as the father’s last name), however, they don’t present the data for an analysis in which identical names were excluded.

14 (Jost, Pelham, & Carvallo, 2002), p. 588.

15 (Orenstein, 2000).

ГЛАВА 18 «РОДИТЕЛИ С МЫШЛЕНИЕМ, ИЗМЕНЕННЫМ НАПОЛОВИНУ»

1 (Moon, Cooper, & Fifer, 1993).

2 (Quinn et al., 2002).

3 For example (Kelly et al., 2007).

4 (Hornik, Risenhoover, & Gunnar, 1987).

5 (Barrett, Campos, & Emde, 1996).

6 (Nash & Krawczyk, 1994). See also (Pomerleau et al., 1990), although this research did not find differences in the youngest age group.

7 (Clearfield & Nelson, 2006).

8 (Donovan, Taylor, & Leavitt, 2007).

9 (Mondschein, Adolph, & Tamis-LeMonda, 2000).

10 For example (Adams et al., 1995; Dunn, Bretherton, & Munn, 1987; Fivush, 1989; Leaper, Anderson, & Sanders, 1998).

11 Several researchers have suggested that implicit attitudes should be especially likely to predict more spontaneous and less controllable behaviours and judgements (e.g., [Strack & Deutsch, 2004], and this is consistent with some experimental work. However, a recent meta-analysis suggests that implicit measures are equally capable of predicting more readily controlled behaviours (Greenwald et al., 2009).

12 (Castelli, De Dea, & Nesdale, 2008), p. 1512.

13 See discussion in (Castelli, Zogmaister, & Tomelleri, 2009). Also (Aboud & Doyle, 1996).

14 (Castelli, Zogmaister, & Tomelleri, 2009).

15 (Tenenbaum & Leaper, 2002). A weak relationship was found in the middle school years. This was a meta-analysis, therefore gender attitudes were assessed in different ways in children and adults.

16. (Weitzman, Birns, & Friend, 1985), p. 897.

17 (Lytton & Romney, 1991).

18 (Freeman, 2007).

19 (Kane, 2006a), quotations from pp. 156, 157, 158, 161, and 161, respectively.

20 (Kane, 2006a), p. 172.

21 In part, this is probably because males are higher status than females, and so it is more acceptable to cross up than to cross down. But as well, there is a fear that feminine interests in boys portends future psychological maladjustment and homosexuality (Martin, 1990; Martin, 2005; Sandnabba & Ahlberg, 1999).

22 (Kane, 2008).

23 (Orenstein, 2000), p. 4.

24 (Alexander, Wilcox, & Woods, 2009).

25 The ‘hot potato’ effect, whereby children find attractive novel toys less appealing when they are labelled as being for the other sex, was demonstrated in four- to five-year-old children by (Martin, Eisenbud, & Rose, 1995).

26 (van de Beek et al., 2009).

27 (Servin, Bohlin, & Berlin, 1999).

28 (Sax, 2006), p. 26.

29 The potential importance of familiarity in preference has been noted by (Zosuls et al., 2009), for example.

30 As found by (Servin, Bohlin, & Berlin, 1999).

31 As found by (Campbell et al., 2000).

32 (Bandura & Bussey, 2004), p. 696.

ГЛАВА 19 «ГЕНДЕРНЫЕ ДЕТЕКТИВЫ»

1 ‘What color for your baby?’ Parents’ 14, no. 3 (March 1939), p. 98. Quoted in (Paoletti, 1997), p. 32.

2 (Hurlbert & Ling, 2007; Alexander, 2003).

3 (Lawson, 2007). Quotations from paras. 4, 5, 8, 8, and 10, respectively.

4 (Paoletti, 1997), pp. 30 and 31, respectively.

5 The salience of gender in the social world, and the active role played by the child in gender development that the salience and importance of gender motivates, has been highlighted by a number of researchers, for example (Arthur et al., 2008; Bem, 1983; Bigler & Liben, 2007; Martin & Halverson, 1981). The material that follows all draws on the insights of Gender Schema Theory and especially Developmental Intergroup Theory.

6 (Gelman, Taylor, & Naguyen, 2004).

7 (Levy & Haaf, 1994).

8 For example (Serbin, Poulin-Dubois, & Eichstedt, 2002), also (Poulin-Dubois et al., 2002), who found that knowledge was seen earlier in girls than in boys.

9 (Zosuls et al., 2009).

10 (Martin, Ruble, & Szkrybalo, 2002; Martin & Halverson, 1981).

11 (Martin & Ruble, 2004), p. 67.

12 (Ruble, Lurye, & Zosuls, 2008), p. 2.

13 (Martin & Ruble, 2004), p. 68.

14 Carol Martin, personal communication, September 9, 2009.

15 (Martin, Eisenbud, & Rose, 1995).

ГЛАВА 20 «ГЕНДЕРНОЕ ОБРАЗОВАНИЕ»

1 (Bem, 1983), p. 611.

2 (Bem, 1989).

3 (Bem, 1983), p. 612.

4 Referring to the title of Sandra Bem’s autobiography (Bem, 1998). At the end of the book the Bems’ children, Jeremy and Emily, then in their early twenties, reflect on their childhood experiences. (In addition to trying to raise ‘gender-aschematic’ children, the Bems also wanted to raise their children in an antihomophobic and sex-positive way.) Both were grateful for what their unconventional rearing had done for them (said Jeremy, ‘I get to be a complete person. That’s what it comes down to’) and were positive about the beliefs their parents had tried to convey to them in their unconventional fashion, although sometimes they disagreed with the details of the implementation. As Sandra Bem acknowledged, there were difficulties for the children in having gender removed as a legitimate source of identity, yet having to live in a culture that remains highly gendered. Both children also noted the difficulty of accepting elements or desires in themselves that were conventionally gendered (for example, to enjoy typically masculine interactions or take pride in being a ‘manly man’ in the case of Jeremy, or to want to be a pretty girl, in the case of Emily). The children ended up pursuing stereotypical interests – Jeremy mathematics and Emily the arts.

5 (Bem, 1983), p. 613.

6 (Rhode, 1997), p. 19.

7 (David, Grace, & Ryan, 2004), p. 142, reference removed, referring to work done by Kay Bussey and David Perry.

8 (Fagot, 1985), see table 3, p. 1102.

9 (Fagot, 1985; Lamb, Easterbrooks, & Holden, 1980; Lamb & Roopnarine, 1979).

10 (Serbin et al., 1979).

11 (Bannerjee & Lintern, 2000).

12 Here I rely on the description of Woodward’s work provided in (Paechter, 2007). Unfortunately, I was unable to locate David Woodward’s thesis (D. Woodward, ‘Nursery class children’s formation of gender perspectives’, Unpublished MPhil thesis, Faculty of Education and Language Studies, Open University, 2003).

13 (Rudman & Glick, 2008), p. 178.

14 (Weitzman et al., 1972), p. 1141.

15 (Gooden & Gooden, 2001; Hamilton et al., 2006).

16 (Novell, 2004) and (Telford, 2003), p. 4.

17 (Rush & La Nauze, 2006).

18 (Turner-Bowker, 1996).

19 (Lamb & Brown, 2006), p. 158.

20 (Frawley, 2008). Quotation is from book blurb, see p. 294.

21 (Diekman & Murnen, 2004).

22 (Evans & Davies, 2000).

23 (Anderson & Hamilton, 2005), p. 149.

24 (Aubrey & Harrison, 2004; Barner, 1999; Leaper et al., 2002; Thompson & Zerbinos, 1995).

25 (Lamb & Brown, 2006), see pp. 64 and 65.

26 (Pike & Jennings, 2005) – 40 percent versus 76.9 percent. The same effect was not found for Harry Potter Lego, although this may have been because many more children had seen the original version of this ad.

27 (Weitzman et al., 1972), p. 1129.

28 (Lamb & Brown, 2006), see pp. 159 and 160. An exception, I think, is Gertrude McFuzz, but as Lamb and Brown note, this girl bird is ‘fancy, vain, and jealous’ (p. 160) – and she also, in the end, has to be rescued by a male character.

29 (Hamilton et al., 2006). (Tepper & Cassidy, 1999) found that females were underrepresented in titles, pictures, and central roles, but contrary to prediction found no differences in emotional language used by male and female characters. (Turner-Bowker, 1996) analysed thirty Caldecott winners and runners up from 1984–1994 and found underrepresentation of females in titles and pictures, although not central roles.

30 (DeLoache, Cassidy, & Carpenter, 1987).

31 (Black et al., 2009; Davis, 2003; Drees & Phye, 2001; Furnham, Abramsky, & Gunter, 1997; Sheldon, 2004).

32 (Götz, 2008).

33 (Smith & Cook, 2008). Both the TV and movie surveys also found that characters were predominantly Caucasian.

34 http://www.thegeenadavisinstitute.org/about_us.php, accessed on October 5, 2009.

35 See brief review in (Miller, Trautner, & Ruble, 2006).

36 (Rudman & Glick, 2008), p. 82.

37 (Fagot, Leinbach, & O’Boyle, 1992), p. 229, referring to work reported in (Leinbach, Hort, & Fagot, 1997).

38 (Leinbach, Hort, & Fagot, 1993).

39 Unpublished work cited in (David, Grace, & Ryan, 2004). Information on the age of the children in the study was provided by the first author (Barbara David, personal communication, June 25, 2009).

ГЛАВА 21 «САМОСОЦИАЛИЗАЦИЯ»

1 (Walker, 2008).

2 (Ruble, Lurye, & Zosuls, 2008), p. 2.

3 (Miller et al., 2009). Appearance was the most commonly used type of stereotype for girls among preschoolers, first, and fourth/fifth graders, although not kindergartners.

4 See summary in (Tajfel & Turner, 1986).

5 (Patterson & Bigler, 2006). See also (Bigler & Liben, 2007).

6 A point made by (Arthur et al., 2008) and (Bem, 1983), for example.

7 A point made by (Rudman & Glick, 2008), p. 73. Interestingly, when children are encouraged to categorise by age (that is, kids versus adults) rather than gender, the adjectives they used to describe boys and girls change (Sani et al., 2003).

8 (Ruble, Lurye, & Zosuls, 2008).

9 (Rudman & Glick, 2008), p. 60, referring to research conducted by (Leinbach, Hort, & Fagot, 1993).

10 Barbara Hort, personal communication, September 17, 2009.

11 Developmental psychologists have pointed out that there are often methodological difficulties with studies that fail to find a relationship between gender knowledge and gender preferences. See (Martin, Ruble, & Szkrybalo, 2002; Miller, Trautner, & Ruble, 2006).

12 (Zosuls et al., 2009).

13 See (Miller, Trautner, & Ruble, 2006), pp. 315 and 316.

14 (Bradbard & Endsley, 1983; Bradbard et al., 1986; Martin, Eisenbud, & Rose, 1995; Masters et al., 1979; Thompson, 1975).

15 (Green, Bigler, & Catherwood, 2004).

16 (Kane, 2006b).

17 (Trautner et al., 2005).

18 This is a joke, rather than a scientific fact.

ЭПИЛОГ «НА СТА-А-АРТ!»

1 This event is described by (Morantz-Sanchez, 1985), pp. 306 and 307. Morantz-Sanchez points out that ‘[i]ronically, women physicians were saying much the same thing as Richard Cabot in their public pronouncements.’ (p. 307).

2 (Baruch, 1915), quotations from paras. 3 and 4, then paras. 7 and 8, respectively.

3 (Dana, 1915), para. 9.

4 Reported in (Nowlan, 2006), para. 9.

5 Quoted in (Morantz-Sanchez, 1985), p. 306.

6 (Dana, 1915), para. 10.

7 (Gould, 1981), pp. 28 and 29.

8 For example (Kay et al., 2004; Lockwood & Kunda, 1997; Shah, 2003; Welnsteln, Przybylskl, & Ryan, 2009).

9 (Kimmel, 2008), p. 4.

10 (Sapolsky, 1997), para. 6.

11 (Kaiser et al., 2009), p. 9, citing the insight of (Fausto-Sterling, 2000). For evidence relating to neuroplasticity, see (Draganski et al., 2004; Maguire et al., 2000).

12 (Kaplan & Rogers, 2003), p. 74.

13 (Fausto-Sterling, 2000), p. 5.

14 For example (Krendl et al., 2008; Wraga et al., 2006). Also (Hausmann et al., 2009) who found that circulating testosterone levels were higher in men who performed cognitive tasks after gender-stereotype priming, compared with controls.

15 (Schweder & Sullivan, 1993), p. 498.

16 (Kimmel, 2008), p. 341.

17 (Bleier, 1986), p. 148.

18 (Kaplan & Rogers, 2003), p. 231.

19 (Davies, 1989), p. x.

20 (Senior, 2009).

 

БИБЛИОГРАФИЯ

Aboud, F. E., & Doyle, A.-B. (1996). Parental and peer influences on children’s racial attitudes. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 20(3/4), 371–383.

Adams, G., Garcia, D. M., Purdie-Vaughns, V., & Steele, C. M. (2006). The detrimental effects of a suggestion of sexism in an instruction situation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(5), 602–615.

Adams, S., Kuebli, J., Boyle, P. A., & Fivush, R. (1995). Gender differences in parent-child conversations about past emotions: A longitudinal investigation. Sex Roles, 33(5/6), 309–323.

Alexander, G. M. (2003). An evolutionary perspective of sex-typed toy preferences: Pink, blue, and the brain. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 32(1), 7–14.

Alexander, G. M., & Hines, M. (2002). Sex differences in response to children’s toys in nonhuman primates (Cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus). Evolution and Human Behavior, 23(6), 467–479.

Alexander, G. M., Wilcox, T., & Woods, R. (2009). Sex differences in infants’ visual interest in toys. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38(3), 427–433.

Allen, K. (2009, July 26). Could crash spell doom for City’s boys’ club? The Observer, 3.

Ambady, N., Shih, M., Kim, A., & Pittinsky, T. L. (2001). Stereotype susceptibility in children: Effects of identity activation on quantitative performance. Psychological Science, 12(5), 385–390.

Ames, D. R., & Kammrath, L. K. (2004). Mind-reading and metacognition: Narcissism, not actual competence, predicts self-estimated ability. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28(3), 187–209.

Anderson, D., & Hamilton, M. (2005). Gender role stereotyping of parents in children’s picture books: The invisible father. Sex Roles, 52(3/4), 145–151.

Andreescu, T., Gallian, J. A., Kane, J. M., & Mertz, J. E. (2008). Cross-cultural analysis of students with exceptional talent in mathematical problem solving. Notices of the American Mathematical Society, 55(10), 1248–1260.

Archer, J., & Coyne, S. M. (2005). An integrated review of indirect, relational, and social aggression. Personality & Social Psychology Review, 9(3), 212–230.

Aronson, J., Lustina, M. J., Good, C., Keough, K., Steele, C. M., & Brown, J. (1999). When white men can’t do math: Necessary and sufficient factors in stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35(1), 29–46.

Arthur, A. E., Bigler, R. S., Liben, L. S., Gelman, S. A., & Ruble, D. N. (2008). Gender stereotyping and prejudice in young children: A developmental intergroup perspective. In S. R. Levy & M. Killen (eds.), Intergroup attitudes and relations in childhood through adulthood (pp. 66–86). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Ashby, J. S., Ryan, M. K., & Haslam, S. A. (2007, March). Legal work and the glass cliff: Evidence that women are preferentially selected to lead problematic cases. William & Mary Journal of Women and the Law, 775–793.

Aubrey, J. S., & Harrison, K. (2004). The gender-role content of children’s favorite television programs and its links to their gender-related perceptions. Media Psychology, 6(2), 111–146.

Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Ashwin, E., Knickmeyer, R., Taylor, K., & Hackett, G. (2009). Fetal testosterone and autistic traits. British Journal of Psychology, 100, 1–22.

Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Ashwin, E., Knickmeyer, R., Taylor, K., Hackett, G., et al. (2009). Fetal testosterone predicts sexually differentiated childhood behaviour in girls and in boys. Psychological Science, 20(2), 144–148.

Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Chapman, E., Knickmeyer, R., Taylor, K., & Hackett, G. (2006). Foetal testosterone and the child systemizing quotient. European Journal of Endocrinology, 155, S123–S130.

Bachelard, M. & Power, L. (2008, November 9). The class divide. Sunday Age, 15.

Banaji, M. R., & Hardin, C. D. (1996). Automatic stereotyping. Psychological Science, 7(3), 136–141.

Banaji, M. R., Nosek, B. A., & Greenwald, A. G. (2004). No place for nostalgia in science: A response to Arkes and Tetlock. Psychological Inquiry, 15(4), 279–310.

Bandura, A., & Bussey, K. (2004). On broadening the cognitive, motivational, and sociostructural scope of theorizing about gender development and functioning: Comment on Martin, Ruble, and Szkrybalo (2002). Psychological Bulletin, 130(5), 691–701.

Bannerjee, R., & Lintern, V. (2000). Boys will be boys: The effect of social evaluation concerns on gender-typing. Social Development, 9(3), 397–408.

Bargh, J., & Williams, E. (2006). The automaticity of social life. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(1), 1–4.

Barner, M. R. (1999). Sex-role stereotyping in FCC-mandated children’s educational television. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 43(4), 551–564.

Barnett, R. & Rivers, C. (2004). Same difference: How gender myths are hurting our relationships, our children, and our jobs. New York: Basic Books.

Barnyard, K., & Lewis, R. (2009). Corporate sexism: The sex industry’s infiltration of the modern workplace. London: The Fawcett Society.

Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

———. (2003). The essential difference: Men, women and the extreme male brain. London: Allen Lane.

———. (2005, August 8). The male condition. New York Times, 15.

———. (2007). Sex differences in mind: Keeping science distinct from social policy. In S. Ceci & W. Williams (eds.), Why aren’t more women in science? Top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 159–172). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

———. (2009). Autism test ‘could hit maths skills’. From BBC News: http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/health/7736196.stm. Accessed February 2, 2009.

Baron-Cohen, S., Knickmeyer, R. C., & Belmonte, M. K. (2005). Sex differences in the brain: Implications for explaining autism. Science, 310, 819–823.

Barres, B. (2006, July 13). Does gender matter? Nature, 442, 133–136.

Barrett, K. C., Campos, J. J., & Emde, R. (1996). Infants’ use of conflicting emotion signals. Cognition and Emotion, 10(2), 113–135.

Baruch, S. (1915, July 4). Why women lack great originality. New York Times, 10.

Batki, A., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Connellan, J., & Ahluwalia, J. (2000). Is there an innate eye gaze module? Evidence from human neonates. Infant Behavior & Development, 23(2), 223–229.

Beilock, S. L., Rydell, R. J., & McConnell, A. R. (2007). Stereotype threat and working memory: Mechanisms, alleviation, and spillover. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136(2), 256–276.

Belkin, L. (2003, October 26). The opt-out revolution. New York Times Magazine, 42.

———. (2008, June 15). When mom and dad share it all. New York Times Magazine, 44.

Bem, S. L. (1983). Gender schema theory and its implications for child development: Raising gender-aschematic children in a gender-schematic society. SIGNS: Journal of Women in Culture & Society, 8(4), 598–616.

———. (1989). Genital knowledge and gender constancy in preschool children. Child Development, 60, 649–662.

——. (1998). An unconventional family. New Haven & London: Yale University Press.

Bennett, C. M., Baird, A. A., Miller, M. B., and Wolford, G. L. (2009). Neural correlates of interspecies perspective taking in the post-mortem Atlantic Salmon: An argument for multiple comparisons correction. [Paper presented at the Organization for Human Brain Mapping 2009 Annual Meeting, San Francisco, CA] NeuroImage, 47(51), S125.

Berenbaum, S. A. (1999). Effects of early androgens on sex-typed activities and interests in adolescents with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Hormones and Behavior, 35, 102–110.

Berenbaum, S. A., & Bailey, J. (2003). Effects on gender identity of prenatal androgens and genital appearance: Evidence from girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 88(3), 1102–1106.

Berenbaum, S. A., & Hines, M. (1992). Early androgens are related to childhood sex-typed toy preferences. Psychological Science, 3(3), 203–206.

Berenbaum, S. A., & Resnick, S. (2007). The seeds of career choices: Prenatal sex hormone effects on psychology sex differences. In S. Ceci & C. Williams (eds.), Why aren’t more women in science? Top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 147–157). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Biernat, M., & Kobrynowicz, D. (1997). Gender-and race-based standards of competence: Lower minimum standards but higher ability standards for devalued groups. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 72(3), 544–557.

Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (2007). Developmental intergroup theory: Explaining and reducing children’s social stereotyping and prejudice. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(3), 162–166.

Bird, C. M., Castelli, F., Malik, O., Frith, U., & Husain, M. (2004). The impact of extensive medial frontal lobe damage on ‘Theory of Mind’ and cognition. Brain, 127, 914–928.

Bishop, K. M., & Wahlsten, D. (1997). Sex differences in the human corpus callosum: Myth or reality? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 21(5), 581–601.

Bittman, M., England, P., Sayer, L., Folbre, N., & Matheson, G. (2003). When does gender trump money? Bargaining and time in household work. American Journal of Sociology, 109(1), 186–214.

Black, K., Marola, J., Littman, A., Chrisler, J., & Neace, W. (2009). Gender and form of cereal box characters: Different medium, same disparity. Sex Roles, 60(11/12), 882–889.

Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention. Child Development, 78(1), 246–263.

Blair, R. J. R. (1996). Morality in the autistic child. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26(5), 571–579.

Blakemore, S.-J., den Ouden, H., Choudhury, S., & Frith, C. (2007). Adolescent development of the neural circuitry for thinking about intentions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 130–139.

Blanton, H., Crocker, J., & Miller, D. T. (2000). The effects of in-group versus out-group social comparison on self-esteem in the context of a negative stereotype. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 519–530.

Bledsoe, A. T. (1856). An essay on liberty and slavery. Philadelphia: J. B. Lippincott & Co.

Bleier, R. (1984). Science and gender: A critique of biology and its theories on women. New York: Pergamon Press.

———. (1986). Sex differences research: Science or belief? In R. Bleier (ed.), Feminist approaches to science (pp. 147–164). New York: Pergamon Press.

Blum, L., & Frieze, C. (2005). The evolving culture of computing: Similarity is the difference. Frontiers, 26(1), 110–125.

Bolino, M. C., & Turnley, W. H. (2003). Counternormative impression management, likeability, and performance ratings: The use of intimidation in an organizational setting. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 24(2), 237–250.

Bowles, H. R., Babcock, L., & Lai, L. (2007). Social incentives for gender differences in the propensity to initiate negotiations: Sometimes it does hurt to ask. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 103, 84–103.

Bradbard, M. R., & Endsley, R. C. (1983). The effects of sex-typed labeling on preschool children’s information-seeking and retention. Sex Roles, 9(2), 247–260.

Bradbard, M. R., Martin, C. L., Endsley, R. C., & Halverson, C. F. (1986). Influence of sex stereotypes on children’s exploration and memory: A competence versus performance distinction. Developmental Psychology, 22(4), 481–486.

Breedlove, S. M., Cooke, B. M., & Jordan, C. L. (1999). The orthodox view of brain sexual differentiation. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 54(1), 8–14.

Brescoll, V., & LaFrance, M. (2004). The correlates and consequences of newspaper reports of research on sex differences. Psychological Science, 15(8), 515–520.

Brescoll, V. L., & Uhlmann, E. L. (2008). Can an angry woman get ahead? Status conferral, gender, and expression of emotion in the workplace. Psychological Science, 19(3), 268–275.

Brines, J. (1994). Economic dependency, gender, and the division of labor at home. American Journal of Sociology, 100(3), 652–688.

Brizendine, L. (2007). The female brain. London: Bantam Press.

Broad, J. (2002). Women philosophers of the seventeenth century. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Broad, J., & Green, K. (2009). A history of women’s political thought in Europe, 1400–1700. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Brosnan, M. J. (2006). Digit ratio and faculty membership: Implications for the relationship between prenatal testosterone and academia. British Journal of Psychology, 97(4), 455–466.

Bruer, J. T. (1997). Education and the brain: A bridge too far. Educational Researcher, 26(8), 4–16.

Bryden, M. P., McManus, I. C., & Bulman-Fleming, M. B. (1994). Evaluating the empirical support for the Geschwind-Behan-Galaburda model of cerebral lateralization. Brain and Cognition, 26(2), 103–167.

Buracas, G. T., Fine, I., & Boynton, G. M. (2005). The relationship between task performance and functional magnetic resonance imaging response. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(12), 3023–3031.

Burnett, S., Bird, G., Moll, J., Frith, C., & Blakemore, S. J. (2009). Development during adolescence of the neural processing of social emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(9), 1736–1750.

Burton, F. D. (1972). The integration of biology and behavior in the socialization of Macaca sylvana of Gibraltar. In F. E. Poirier (ed.), Primate socialization (pp. 29–62). New York: Random House.

———. (1977). Ethology and the development of sex and gender identity in non-human primates. Acta Biotheoretica, 26(1), 1–18.

———. (1992). The social group as information unit: Cognitive behaviour, cultural processes. In F. D. Burton (ed.), Social processes and mental abilities in non-human primates: Evidences from longitudinal field studies (pp. 31–60). Lewiston/Queenston/Lampeter: The Edwin Mellen Press.

Butler, D., & Geis, F. L. (1990). Nonverbal affect responses to male and female leaders: Implications for leadership evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(1), 48–59.

Cadinu, M., Maass, A., Frigerio, S., Impagliazzo, L., & Latinotti, S. (2003). Stereotype threat: The effect of expectancy on performance. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33(2), 267–285.

Cadinu, M., Maass, A., Rosabianca, A., & Kiesner, J. (2005). Why do women underperform under stereotype threat? Evidence for the role of negative thinking. Psychological Science, 16(7), 572–578.

Cahill, L. (2006). Why sex matters for neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(6), 477–484.

Cameron, D. (2007). The myth of Mars and Venus: Do men and women really speak different languages? Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Campbell, A., Shirley, L., Heywood, C., & Crook, C. (2000). Infants’ visual preferences for sex-congruent babies, children, toys and activities: A longitudinal study. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 18, 479–498.

Castelli, L., De Dea, C., & Nesdale, D. (2008). Learning social attitudes: Children’s sensitivity to the nonverbal behaviors of adult models during interracial interactions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(11), 1504–1513.

Castelli, L., Zogmaister, C., & Tomelleri, S. (2009). The transmission of racial attitudes within the family. Developmental Psychology, 45(2), 586–591.

Ceci, S. J., Williams, W. M., & Barnett, S. M. (2009). Women’s underrepresentation in science: Sociocultural and biological considerations. Psychological Bulletin, 135(2), 218–261.

Chalfin, M. C., Murphy, E. R., & Karkazis, K. A. (2008). Women’s neuroethics? Why sex matters for neuroethics. American Journal of Bioethics, 8(1), 1–2.

Chapman, E., Baron-Cohen, S., Auyeung, B., Knickmeyer, R., Taylor, K., & Hackett, G. (2006). Fetal testosterone and empathy: Evidence from the Empathy Quotient (EQ) and the ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’ test. Social Neuroscience, 1(2), 135–148.

Charles, M., & Bradley, K. (2009). Indulging our gendered selves? Sex segregation by field of study in 44 countries. American Journal of Sociology, 114(4), 924–976.

Chatard, A., Guimond, S., & Selimbegovic, L. (2007). ‘How good are you in math?’ The effect of gender stereotypes on students’ recollection of their school marks. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(6), 1017–1024.

Cherney, I. D. (2008). Mom, let me play more computer games: They improve my mental rotation skills. Sex Roles, 59, 776–786.

Cheryan, S., Plaut, V. C., Davies, P. G., & Steele, C. M. (2009). Ambient belonging: How stereotypical cues impact gender participation in computer science. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 97(6), 1045–1060.

Choudhury, S., Nagel, S. K., & Slaby, J. (2009). Critical neuroscience: Linking neuroscience and society through critical practice. BioSocieties, 4, 61–77.

Clarke, E. H. (1873). Sex in education: Or, a fair chance for girls. Boston: James R. Osgood & Company. Released by The Project Gutenberg, June 5, 2006.

Clearfield, M. W., & Nelson, N. M. (2006). Sex differences in mothers’ speech and play behavior with 6-, 9-, and 14-month-old infants. Sex Roles, 54(1/2), 127–137.

Clements, A. M., Rimrodt, S. L., Abel, J. R., Blankner, J. G., Mostofsky, S. H., Pekar, J. J., et al. (2006). Sex differences in cerebral laterality of language and visuospatial processing. Brain and Language, 98(2), 150–158.

Coleman, J. M., & Hong, Y. Y. (2008). Beyond nature and nurture: The influence of lay gender theories on self-stereotyping. Self and Identity, 7(1), 34–53.

Collins, G. (1982, May 17). New perspectives on father and his role. From the New York Times online: http://www.nytimes.com/1982/05/17/style/new-perspectives-on-father-and-his-role.html, accessed March 7, 2009.

Connellan, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Batki, A., & Ahluwalia, J. (2000). Sex differences in human neonatal social perception. Infant Behavior & Development, 23, 113–118.

Correll, S. J. (2004). Constraints into preferences: Gender, status, and emerging career aspirations. American Sociological Review, 69(1), 93–113.

———. (2001). Gender and the career choice process: The role of biased self-assessment. American Journal of Sociology, 106(6), 1691–1730.

Correll, S. J., Benard, S., & Paik, I. (2007). Getting a job: Is there a motherhood penalty? American Journal of Sociology, 112(5), 1297–1338.

Costa, P., Jr., Terracciano, A., & McCrae, R. R. (2001). Gender differences in personality traits across cultures: Robust and surprising findings. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 81(2), 322–331.

Croizet, J.-C., Després, G., Gauzins, M.-E., Huguet, P., Leyens, J.-P., & Méot, A. (2004). Stereotype threat undermines intellectual performance by triggering a disruptive mental load. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(6), 721–731.

Crosby, F. J., Williams, J. C., & Biernat, M. (2004). The maternal wall. Journal of Social Issues, 60(4), 675–682.

Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2004). When professionals become mothers, warmth doesn’t cut the ice. Journal of Social Issues, 60(4), 701–718.

Dakss, B. (2005, March 13). Intellectual gender gap? Kaledin on debate reignited by Harvard president’s comments. From CBS News.com: http://www.cbsnews.com/stories/2005/03/14/sunday/main679829.shtml. Accessed on November 24, 2009.

Dana, C. L. (1915, June 27). Suffrage a cult of self and sex. New York Times, 14, (online archive).

Danaher, K., & Crandall, C. S. (2008). Stereotype threat in applied settings re-examined. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 38(6), 1639–1655.

Dar-Nimrod, I., & Heine, S. J. (2006). Exposure to scientific theories affects women’s math performance. Science, 314(5798), 435.

Dasgupta, N., & Asgari, S. (2004). Seeing is believing: Exposure to counterstereotypic women leaders and its effect on the malleability of automatic gender stereotyping. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 642–658.

David, B., Grace, D., & Ryan, M. K. (2004). The gender wars: A self-categorisation theory perspective on the development of gender identity. In M. Bennett & F. Sani (eds.), The development of the social self (pp. 135–157). Hove, UK, and New York: Psychology Press.

Davies, B. (1989). Frogs and snails and feminist tales: Preschool children and gender. Sydney: Allen & Unwin.

Davies, P. G., Spencer, S. J., Quinn, D. M., & Gerhardstein, R. (2002). Consuming images: How television commercials that elicit stereotype threat can restrain women academically and professionally. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(12), 1615–1628.

Davies, P. G., Spencer, S. J., & Steele, C. M. (2005). Clearing the air: Identity safety moderates the effects of stereotype threat on women’s leadership aspirations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(2), 276–287.

Davis, M. H., & Kraus, L. A. (1997). Personality and empathic accuracy. In W. J. Ickes (ed.), Empathic accuracy (pp. 144–168). New York: The Guilford Press.

Davis, S. N. (2003). Sex stereotypes in commercials targeted toward children: A content analysis. Sociological Spectrum, 23, 407–424.

———. (2007). Gender ideology construction from adolescence to young adulthood. Social Science Research, 36, 1021–1041.

Davison, H. K., & Burke, M. J. (2000). Sex discrimination in simulated employment contexts: A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 56(2), 225–248.

De Vries, G. J. (2004). Sex differences in adult and developing brains: Compensation, compensation, compensation. Endocrinology, 145(3), 1063–1068.

DeLoache, J. S., Cassidy, D. J., & Carpenter, C. J. (1987). The three bears are all boys: Mothers’ gender labeling of neutral picture book characters. Sex Roles, 17(3/4), 163–178.

Demos, J. (1982). The changing faces of fatherhood: A new exploration in American family history. In S. Cath, A. Gurwitt, & J. Ross (eds.), Father and child: Developmental and clinical perspectives (pp. 425–445). Boston: Little, Brown.

Deutsch, F. (1999). Halving it all: How equally shared parenting works. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Devos, T., Diaz, P., Viera, E., & Dunn, R. (2007). College education and motherhood as components of self-concept: Discrepancies between implicit and explicit assessments. Self and Identity, 6(2/3), 256–277.

Diamond, M. C. (1991). Hormonal effects on the development of cerebral lateralization. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 16(1–3), 121–129.

Diekman, A. B., & Murnen, S. K. (2004). Learning to be little women and little men: The inequitable gender equality of nonsexist children’s literature. Sex Roles, 50(5/6), 373–385.

Dietrich, T., Krings, T., Neulen, J., Willmes, K., Erberich, S., Thron, A., & Sturm, W. (2001). Effects of blood estrogen level on cortical activation patterns during cognitive activation as measured by functional MRI. NeuroImage, 13(3), 425–432.

Donovan, W., Taylor, N., & Leavitt, L. (2007). Maternal sensory sensitivity and response bias in detecting change in infant facial expressions: Maternal self-efficacy and infant gender labeling. Infant Behavior and Development, 30(3), 436–452.

Dorr, R. C. (1915, September 19). Is woman biologically barred from success? New York Times, SM15 (online archive).

Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Busch, V., Schuierer, G., Bogdahn, U., & May, A. (2004). Neuroplasticity: Changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature, 427(6972), 311–312.

Drake, J. (1696). An essay in defence of the female sex. In which are inserted the characters of a pedant, a squire, a vertuoso, a poetaster, a city-critick, &c. In a letter to a lady. Written by a lady. London: Printed for A. Roper, E. Wilkinson, and R. Clavel.

Drees, D. D., & Phye, G. D. (2001). Gender representation in children’s language arts computer software. Journal of Educational Research, 95(1), 49–55.

Dugan, E. (2008, March 31). Women’s group calls for the end to city’s lap-dancing culture. The Independent, 12.

Dunn, J., Bretherton, I., & Munn, P. (1987). Conversations about feeling states between mothers and their young children. Developmental Psychology, 23(1), 132–139.

Dweck, C. S. (2007). Is math a gift? Beliefs that put females at risk. In S. Ceci & W. Williams (eds.), Why aren’t more women in science? Top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 47–55). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Dziobek, I., Rogers, K., Fleck, S., Bahnemann, M., Heekeren, H. R., Wolf, O. T., & Convit, A. (2008). Dissociation of cognitive and emotional empathy in adults with Asperger syndrome using the Multifaceted Empathy Test (MET). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(3), 464–473.

Edge (2005a). The assortative mating theory: A talk with Simon Baron-Cohen. http://www.edge.org/3rd_culture/baron-cohen05/baron-cohen05_index.html. Accessed September 2, 2009.

Edge (2005b, May 16). The science of gender and science. Pinker vs. Spelke. A debate. http://www.edge.org/3rd_culture/debate05/debate05_index.html. Accessed January 20, 2009.

Eisenberg, N., & Lennon, R. (1983). Sex differences in empathy and related capacities. Psychological Bulletin, 94(1), 100–131.

Evans, L., & Davies, K. (2000). No sissy boys here: A content analysis of the representation of masculinity in elementary school reading textbooks. Sex Roles, 42(3/4), 255–270.

Fabes, R. A., & Eisenberg, N. (1998). Meta-analyses of age and sex differences in children’s and adolescents’ prosocial behavior. (Manuscript partially published in: Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1998). Prosocial development. In W. Damon (ed.), Handbook of Child Development. Available at http://www.public.asu.edu/~rafabes/meta.pdf.)

Fagot, B., Leinbach, M., & O’Boyle, C. (1992). Gender labeling, gender stereotyping, and parenting behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 28(2), 225–230.

Fagot, B. I. (1985). Beyond the reinforcement principle: Another step toward understanding sex role development. Developmental Psychology, 21(6), 1097–1104.

Fara, P. (2005). Scientists anonymous: Great stories of women in science. Cambridge: Icon Books.

Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Simion, F., & Johnson, M. H. (2002). Eye contact detection in humans from birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 99(14), 9602–9605.

Fausto-Sterling, A. (1985). Myths of gender: Biological theories about women and men (2nd ed.). New York: Basic Books.

———. (2000). Sexing the body: Gender politics and the construction of sexuality. New York: Basic Books.

Feingold, A. (1994). Gender differences in variability in intellectual abilities: A cross-cultural perspective. Sex Roles, 30(1/2), 81–92.

Feng, J., Spence, I., & Pratt, J. (2007). Playing an action video game reduces gender differences in spatial cognition. Psychological Science, 18(10), 850–855.

Fine, A. (1990). Taking the devil’s advice. London: Viking.

Fine, C. (2006). A mind of its own: How your brain distorts and deceives. New York: W. W. Norton.

———. (2008). Will working mothers’ brains explode? The popular new genre of neurosexism. Neuroethics, 1(1), 69–72.

Finegan, J. K., Niccols, G. A., & Sitarenios, G. (1992). Relations between prenatal testosterone levels and cognitive abilities at 4 years. Developmental Psychology, 28(6), 1075–1089.

Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 77–83.

Fivush, R. (1989). Exploring sex differences in the emotional content of mother-child conversations about the past. Sex Roles, 20(11/12), 675–691.

Frawley, T. J. (2008). Gender schema and prejudicial recall: How children misremember, fabricate, and distort gendered picture book information. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 22(3), 291–303.

Freeman, N. K. (2007). Preschoolers’ perceptions of gender appropriate toys and their parents’ beliefs about genderized behaviors: Miscommunication, mixed messages, or hidden truths? Early Childhood Education Journal, 34(5), 357–366.

Freese, J. L. & Amaral, D. G. (2009). Neuroanatomy of the primate amygdala. In P. J. Whalen & E. A. Phelps (eds.), The human amygdala (pp. 3–42). New York: Guilford Press.

Friston, K. J., & Price, C. J. (2001). Dynamic representations and generative models of brain function. Brain Research Bulletin, 54(3), 275–285.

Fullagar, C. J., Canan Sumer, H., Sverke, M., & Slick, R. (2003). Managerial sex-role stereotyping, a cross cultural analysis. International Journal of Cross Cultural Management, 3(1), 93–107.

Furnham, A., Abramsky, S., & Gunter, B. (1997). A cross-cultural content analysis of children’s television advertisements. Sex Roles, 37(1/2), 91–99.

Galinsky, A. D., Wang, C. S., & Ku, G. (2008). Perspective-takers behave more stereotypically. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(2), 404–419.

Galpin, V. (2002). Women in computing around the world. SIGCSE Bulletin, 34(2), 94–100.

Garner, H. (2004). Joe Cinque’s consolation: A true story of death, grief and the law. Sydney: Pan Macmillan.

Garner, R. (2008, November 18). Single-sex schools ‘are the future’. From The Independent online: http://www.independent.co.uk/news/education/education-news/singlesex-schools-are-the-future-1023105.html, accessed on May 22, 2009.

Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude on change. Psychological Bulletin, 132(5), 692–731.

Gelman, S. A., Taylor, M. G., & Naguyen, S. P. (2004). III. How children and mothers express gender essentialism. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 69(1), 33–63.

Geschwind, N., & Behan, P. (1982). Left-handedness: Association with immune disease, migraine, and developmental learning disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 79, 5097–5100.

Gharibyan, H. (2007). Gender gap in computer science: Studying its absence in one former Soviet Republic. 2007 American Society for Engineering Education Conference Proceedings, Honolulu, Hawaii.

Gharibyan, H., & Gunsaulus, S. (2006). Gender gap in computer science does not exist in one former Soviet Republic: Results of a study. Annual Joint Conference Integrating Technology into Computer Science Education. Proceedings of the 11th annual SIGCSE conference on Innovation and Technology in computer science education. Bologna, Italy, pp. 222–226.

Giedd, J. N., Clasen, L. S., Lenroot, R., Greenstein, D., Wallace, G. L., Ordaz, S. (2006). Puberty-related influences on brain development. Molecular and Cellular Endocrinology, 254/255, 154–162.

Gilmore, J. H., Lin, W., Prastawa, M. W., Looney, C. B., Vetsa, Y. S. K., Knickmeyer, R. C., et al. (2007). Regional gray matter growth, sexual dimorphism, and cerebral asymmetry in the neonatal brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(6), 1255–1260.

Gisborne, T. (1797). An enquiry into the duties of the female sex. London: Printed for T. Cadell and W. Davies in the Strand.

Gitau, R., Adams, D., Fisk, N. M., & Glover, V. (2005). Fetal plasma testosterone correlates positively with cortisol. Archives of Disease in Childhood, Fetal and Neonatal Edition, 90, F166–F169.

Gladwell, M. (2008). Outliers: The story of success. Camberwell, Victoria, Australia: The Penguin Group.

Glick, P., & Fiske, S. (2007). Sex discrimination: The psychological approach. In F. Crosby, M. Stockdale, & S. Ropp (eds.), Sex discrimination in the workplace: Multidisciplinary perspectives (pp. 155–187). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.

Golombok, S., & Rust, J. (1993). The pre-school activities inventory: A standardised assessment of gender role in children. Psychological Assessment, 5(2), 131–136.

Gonzalez, A. Q., & Koestner, R. (2005). Parental preference for sex of newborn as reflected in positive affect in birth announcements. Sex Roles, 52(5/6), 407–411.

Good, C., Aronson, J., & Harder, J. A. (2008). Problems in the pipeline: Stereotype threat and women’s achievement in high-level math courses. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 29(1), 17–28.

Good, C., Aronson, J., & Inzlicht, M. (2003). Improving adolescents’ standardized test performance: An intervention to reduce the effects of stereotype threat. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 24(6), 645–662.

Good, C., Rattan, A., & Dweck, C. (unpublished manuscript). Why do women opt out? Sense of belonging and women’s representation in mathematics.

Gooden, A. M., & Gooden, M. A. (2001). Gender representation in notable children’s picture books: 1995–1999. Sex Roles, 45(1/2), 89–101.

Gooren, L. (2006). The biology of human psychosexual differentiation. Hormones and Behavior, 50, 589–601.

Gorman, E. H., & Kmec, J. A. (2007). We (have to) try harder: gender and required work effort in Britain and the United States. Gender and Society, 21(6), 828–856.

Götz, M. (2008). Girls and boys and television: A few reminders for more gender sensitivity in children’s TV. Germany: Internationales Zentralinstitut für das Jugend – und Bildungsfernsehen / International Central Institute for Youth and Educational Television (IZI).

Gould, S. (1981). The mismeasure of man. London: Penguin Books.

Graham, T., & Ickes, W. (1997). When women’s intuition isn’t greater than men’s. In W. Ickes (ed.), Empathic accuracy (pp. 117–143). New York and London: The Guilford Press.

Gray, J. (2008). Why Mars and Venus collide. London: Harper Collins.

Green, V. A., Bigler, R., & Catherwood, D. (2004). The variability and flexibility of gender-typed toy play: A close look at children’s behavioral responses to counterstereotypic models. Sex Roles, 51(7/8), 371–386.

Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., & Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(1), 17–41.

Grimshaw, G. M., Sitarenios, G., & Finegan, J. A. K. (1995). Mental rotation at 7 years – Relations with prenatal testosterone levels and spatial play experiences. Brain and Cognition, 29(1), 85–100.

Grön, G., Wunderlich, A. P., Spitzer, M., Tomczak, R., & Riepe, M. W. (2000). Brain activation during human navigation: Gender-different neural networks as substrate of performance. Nature Neuroscience, 3(4), 404–408.

Grossi, G. (2008). Science or belief? Bias in sex differences research. In C. Badaloni, A. Drace, O. Gia, C. Levorato, & F. Vidotto (eds.), Under-representation of women in science and technology (pp. 93–106). Padova, Italy: Cleup.

Guimond, S. (2008). Psychological similarities and differences between women and men across cultures. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(1), 494–510.

Guiso, L., Monte, F., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. (2008). Culture, gender, and math. Science, 320(5880), 1164–1165.

Gupta, V. K., & Bhawe, N. M. (2007). The influence of proactive personality and stereotype threat on women’s entrepreneurial intentions. Journal of Leadership and Organizational Studies, 13(4), 73–85.

Gur, R. C., Alsop, D., Glahn, D., Petty, R., Swanson, C. L., Maldjian, J. A., et al. (2000). An fMRI study of sex differences in regional activation to a verbal and a spatial task. Brain and Language, 74(2), 157–170.

Gur, R. C., & Gur, R. E. (2007). Neural substrates for sex differences in cognition. In Why aren’t more women in science? Top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 189–198). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Gur, R. C., Turetsky, B. I., Matsui, M., Yan, M., Bilker, W., Hughett, P., & Gur, R. E. (1999). Sex differences in gray and white brain matter in healthy young adults: Correlations with cognitive performance. Journal of Neuroscience, 19(10), 4065–4072.

Gürer, D. W. (2002a). Pioneering women in computer science. SIGCSE Bulletin, 34(2), 175–180.

———. (2002b). Women in computing history. SIGCSE Bulletin, 34(2), 116–120.

Gurian Institute, Bering, S., & Goldberg, A. (2009). It’s a baby girl! The unique wonder and special nature of your daughter from pregnancy to two years. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Gurian, M. (2004). What could he be thinking? A guide to the mysteries of a man’s mind. London: Element.

Gurian, M., & Annis, B. (2008). Leadership and the sexes: Using gender science to create success in business. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Gurian, M., Henley, P., & Trueman, T. (2001). Boys and girls learn differently! A guide for teachers and parents. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Gurian, M., & Stevens, K. (2004, November). With boys and girls in mind. Educational Leadership, 21–26.

———. (2005). The minds of boys: Saving our sons from falling behind in school and life. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Gutek, B. A., & Done, R. S. (2001). Sexual harassment. In R. K. Unger (ed.), Handbook of the psychology of women and gender (pp. 367–387). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Haier, R. J., Jung, R. E., Yeo, R. A., Head, K., & Alkire, M. T. (2005). The neuroanatomy of general intelligence: Sex matters. NeuroImage, 25, 320–327.

Haier, R. J., Siegal, B. V., MacLachlan, A., Soderling, E., Lottenberg, S., & Buchsbaum, M. S. (1992). Regional glucose metabolic changes after learning a complex visuospatial/motor task: A positron emission tomographic study. Brain Research, 570, 134–143.

Halari, R., Sharma, T., Hines, M., Andrew, C., Simmons, A., & Kumari, V. (2006). Comparable fMRI activity with differential behavioural performance on mental rotation and overt verbal fluency tasks in healthy men and women. Experimental Brain Research, 169(1), 1–14.

Hall, G. B. C., Witelson, S. F., Szechtman, H., & Nahmias, C. (2004). Sex differences in functional activation patterns revealed by increased emotion processing demands. NeuroReport, 15(2), 219–223.

Hall, J. A. (1978). Gender effects in decoding nonverbal cues. Psychological Bulletin, 85(4), 845–857.

———. (1984). Nonverbal sex differences: Communication accuracy and expressive style. Baltimore & London: The Johns Hopkins University Press.

Halpern, D. F., Benbow, C. P., Geary, D. C., Gur, R. C., Hyde, J. S., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2007). The science of sex differences in science and mathematics. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 8(1), 1–51.

Hamilton, C. (2004). Growth fetish. London: Pluto Press.

Hamilton, M. C., Anderson, D., Broaddus, M., & Young, K. (2006). Gender stereotyping and under-representation of female characters in 200 popular children’s picture books: A twenty-first century update. Sex Roles, 55(11/12), 757–765.

Hanlon, H. W., Thatcher, R. W., & Cline, M. J. (1999). Gender differences in the development of EEG coherence in normal children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 16(3), 479–506.

Harrington, G. S., & Farias, S. T. (2008). Sex differences in language processing: Functional MRI methodological considerations. Journal of Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 27(6), 1221–1228.

Haslam, S. A., & Ryan, M. K. (2008). The road to the glass cliff: Differences in the perceived suitability of men and women for leadership positions in succeeding and failing organizations. The Leadership Quarterly, 19(5), 530–546.

Haslanger, S. (2008). Changing the ideology and culture of philosophy: Not by reason (alone). Hypatia, 23(2), 210–223.

Hassett, J. M., Siebert, E. R., & Wallen, K. (2008). Sex differences in rhesus monkey toy preferences parallel those of children. Hormones and Behavior, 54(3), 359–364.

Hausmann, M., Schoofs, D., Rosenthal, H. E. S., & Jordan, K. (2009). Interactive effects of sex hormones and gender stereotypes on cognitive sex differences – A psychobiological approach. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 389–401.

Healy, M. (2006a, May 8). Hunter vs. gatherer (and then some). Los Angeles Times, F12.

———. (2006b, July 23). In mental juggling, gender makes a difference. Telegraph (Nashua, NH), Health section.

Heilman, M. E. (2001). Description and prescription: How gender stereotypes prevent women’s ascent up the organizational ladder. Journal of Social Issues, 57(4), 657–674.

Heilman, M. E., & Chen, J. J. (2005). Same behavior, different consequences: Reactions to men’s and women’s altruistic citizenship behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(3), 431–441.

Heilman, M. E., Wallen, A. S., Fuchs, D., & Tamkins, M. M. (2004). Penalties for success: Reactions to women who succeed at male gender-typed tasks. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(3), 416–427.

Herman, R. A., Measday, M. A., & Wallen, K. (2003). Sex differences in interest in infants in juvenile rhesus monkeys: Relationship to prenatal androgens. Hormones and Behavior, 43, 573–583.

Hersch, J. (2006). Sex discrimination in the labor market. Foundations and Trends in Microeconomics, 2(4), 281–361.

Hess, B. B. (1990). Beyond dichotomy: Drawing distinctions and embracing differences. Sociological Forum, 5(1), 75–93.

Hewlett, S., Luce, C. B., & Servon, L. J. (2008, December). [Response to letter]. Harvard Business Review, 114.

Hewlett, S. A., Luce, C. B., Servon, L. J., Sherbin, L., Shiller, P., Sosnovich, E., & Sumberg, K. (2008). The Athena factor: Reversing the brain drain in science, engineering, and technology. Boston: Harvard Business Review.

Hines, M. (2004). Brain gender. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press.

———. (2006a). Do sex differences in cognition cause the shortage of women in science? In S. Ceci & W. Williams (eds.), Why aren’t more women in science? Top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 101–112). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

———. (2006b). Prenatal testosterone and gender-related behaviour. European Journal of Endocrinology, 155, S115–S121.

Hines, M., & Alexander, G. (2008). Monkeys, girls, boys and toys: A confirmation. Letter regarding ‘Sex differences in toy preferences: Striking parallels between monkeys and humans’. Hormones and Behavior, 54, 478–479.

Hines, M., Fane, B. A., Pasterski, V. L., Mathews, G. A., Conway, G. S., & Brook, C. (2003). Spatial abilities following prenatal androgen abnormality: Targeting and mental rotations performance in individuals with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 28, 1010–1026.

Hines, M., Golombok, S., Rust, J., Johnston, K., Golding, J., and the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children Study Team (2002). Testosterone during pregnancy and gender role behavior of preschool children: A longitudinal, population study. Child Development, 73(6), 1678–1687.

Hinze, S. W. (2004). ‘Am I being over-sensitive?’ Women’s experience of sexual harassment during medical training. Health: An Interdisciplinary Journal for the Social Study of Health, Illness and Medicine, 8(1), 101–127.

Hochschild, A. R. (1990). The second shift. New York: Avon Books.

Hoff Sommers, C. (2008). Why can’t a woman be more like a man? From The American online: March/April issue. http://www.american.com/archive/2008/march-april-magazine-contents/why-can2019t-a-woman-be-more-like-a-man, accessed on March 19, 2008.

Hogg, M. A., & Turner, J. C. (1987). Intergroup behaviour, self-stereotyping and the salience of social categories. British Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 325–340.

Hollingworth, L. S. (1914). Variability as related to sex differences in achievement: A critique. American Journal of Sociology, 21, 510–530.

Horne, M. (2007, July 29). Gender row cooking up in classroom. Scotland on Sunday, 7.

Hornik, R., Risenhoover, N., & Gunnar, M. (1987). The effects of maternal positive, neutral, and negative affective communications on infant responses to new toys. Child Development, 58, 937–944.

Houck, M. (2009). Is forensic science a gateway for women in science? Forensic Science Policy and Management, 1, 65–69.

Hughes, C., & Cutting, A. L. (1999). Nature, nurture, and individual differences in early understanding of mind. Psychological Science, 10(5), 429–432.

Hughes, C., Jaffee, S. R., Happé, F., Taylor, A., Caspi, A., & Moffit, T. E. (2005). Origins of differences in theory of mind: From nature to nurture? Child Development, 76(2), 356–370.

Huguet, P., & Régner, I. (2007). Stereotype threat among schoolgirls in quasi-ordinary classroom circumstances. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(3), 545–560.

Hurlbert, A. C., & Ling, Y. (2007). Biological components of sex differences in color preference. Current Biology, 17(16), R623–R625.

Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60(6), 581–592.

Hyde, J. S., Lindberg, S. M., Linn, M. C., Ellis, A. B., & Williams, C. C. (2008). Gender similarities characterize math performance. Science 321, 494–495.

Hyde, J. S., & Mertz, J. E. (2009). Gender, culture, and mathematics performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(22), 8801–8807.

Ickes, W. (2003). Everyday mind reading: Understanding what other people think and feel. Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books.

Ickes, W., Gesn, P. R., & Graham, T. (2000). Gender differences in empathic accuracy: Differential ability or differential motivation? Personal Relationships, 7, 95–109.

Ihnen, S. K. Z., Church, J. A., Petersen, S. E., & Schlaggar, B. L. (2009). Lack of generalizability of sex differences in the fMRI BOLD activity associated with language processing in adults. NeuroImage, 45(3), 1020–1032.

Iidaka, T., Okada, T., Murata, T., Omori, M., Kosaka, H., Sadato, N., & Yonekura, Y. (2002). Age-related differences in the medial temporal lobe responses to emotional faces as revealed by fMRI. Hippocampus, 12, 352–362.

Im, K., Lee, J.-M., Lyttelton, O., Kim, S. H., Evans, A. C., & Kim, S. I. (2008). Brain size and cortical structure in the adult human brain. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 2181–2191.

Inzlicht, M., & Ben-Zeev, T. (2000). A threatening intellectual environment: Why females are susceptible to experiencing problem-solving deficits in the presence of males. Psychological Science, 11(5), 365–371.

Itani, J. (1959). Paternal care in the wild Japanese monkey, Macaca fuscata fuscata. Primates, 2(1), 61–93.

James, K. (1993). Conceptualizing self with in-group stereotypes: Context and esteem precursors. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19(1), 117–121.

Jeffreys, S. (2008). Keeping women down and out: The strip club boom and the reinforcement of male dominance. SIGNS: Journal of Women in Culture and Society, 34(1), 151–173.

Johns, M., Inzlicht, M., & Schmader, T. (2008). Stereotype threat and executive resource depletion: Examining the influence of emotion regulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(4), 691–705.

Johnson, W., Carothers, A., & Deary, I. J. (2008). Sex differences in variability in general intelligence. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(6), 518–531.

Jolls, C. (2002). Is there a glass ceiling? Harvard Women’s Law Journal, 25, 1–18.

Jordan, K., Wüstenberg, T., Heinze, H.-J., Peters, M., & Jäncke, L. (2002). Women and men exhibit different cortical activation patterns during mental rotation tasks. Neuropsychologia, 40(13), 2397–2408.

Josephs, R. A., Newman, M. L., Brown, R. P., & Beer, J. M. (2003). Status, testosterone, and human intellectual performance: Stereotype threat as status concern. Psychological Science, 14(2), 158–163.

Jost, J., & Hunyady, O. (2002). The psychology of system justification and the palliative function of ideology. European Review of Social Psychology, 13, 111–153.

Jost, J. T., Pelham, B. W., & Carvallo, M. R. (2002). Non-conscious forms of system justification: Implicit and behavioral preferences for higher status groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 586–602.

Jürgensen, M., Hiort, O., Holterhus, P.-M., & Thyen, U. (2007). Gender role behavior in children with XY karyotype and disorders of sex development. Hormones and Behavior, 51(3), 443–453.

Kaiser, A., Haller, S., Schmitz, S., & Nitsch, C. (2009). On sex/gender related similarities and differences in fMRI language research. Brain Research Reviews, 61(2): 49–59.

Kane, E. W. (2006a). ‘No way my boys are going to be like that!’: Responses to children’s gender nonconformity. Gender and Society, 20(149), 149–176.

———. (2006b). ‘We put it down in front of him, and he just instinctively knew what to do’: Biological determinism in parents’ beliefs about the origins of gendered childhoods. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Sociological Association, Montreal, Canada.

———. (2008). Called to account: Parents and children doing gender in everyday interactions. Paper presented at the Eastern Sociological Society Annual Meeting, New York, NY.

———. (2009). ‘I wanted a soul mate:’ Gendered anticipation and frameworks of accountability in parents’ preferences for sons and daughters. Symbolic Interaction, 34(4), 372–389.

Kaplan, G., & Rogers, L. (1994). Race and gender fallacies: The paucity of biological determinist explanations of difference. In E. Tobach & B. Rosoff (eds.), Challenging racism and sexism: Alternatives to genetic explanations (pp. 66–92). New York: The Feminist Press.

Kaplan, G., & Rogers, L. (2003). Gene worship: Moving beyond the nature/nurture debate over genes, brain, and gender. New York: Other Press.

Kay, A., Wheeler, S., Bargh, J., & Ross, L. (2004). Material priming: The influence of mundane physical objects on situational construal and competitive behavioral choice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 95, 83–96.

Kelly, D. J., Liu, S., Ge, L., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., Lee, K., et al. (2007). Cross-race preferences for same-race faces extend beyond the African versus Caucasian contrast in 3-month-old infants. Infancy, 11(1), 87–95.

Kemper, T. (1990). Social structure and testosterone: Explorations of the socio-biosocial chain. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.

Kiefer, A. K., & Sekaquaptewa, D. (2007). Implicit stereotypes and women’s math performance: How implicit gender-math stereotypes influence women’s susceptibility to stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(5), 825–832.

Kilbourne, B., England, P., Farkas, G., Beron, K., & Weir, D. (1994). Returns to skill, compensating differentials, and gender bias: Effects of occupational characteristics on the wages of white women and men. American Journal of Sociology, 100(3), 689–719.

Killgore, W. D. S., Oki, M., & Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (2001). Sex-specific developmental changes in amygdala responses to affective faces. NeuroReport, 12(2), 427–433.

Killgore, W. D. S., Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (2001). Sex differences in amygdala activation during the perception of facial affect. NeuroReport, 12(11), 2543–2547.

Kimmel, M. S. (2004). The gendered society (2nd ed.). New York and Oxford: Oxford University Press.

———. (2008). The gendered society (3rd ed.). New York and Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Kimura, D. (1999). Sex and cognition. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

———. (2005, February 1). Hysteria trumps academic freedom. Vancouver Sun, p. A13.

Kitayama, S., & Cohen, D. (2007). Preface. In S. Kitayama & D. Cohen (eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology. New York: The Guilford Press.

Klein, K. J. K., & Hodges, S. D. (2001). Gender differences, motivation, and empathic accuracy: When it pays to understand. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(6), 720–730.

Knickmeyer, R., Baron-Cohen, S., Fane, B. A., Wheelwright, S., Mathews, G. A., Conway, G. S., et al. (2006). Androgens and autistic traits: A study of individuals with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Hormones and Behavior, 50, 148–153.

Knickmeyer, R., Baron-Cohen, S., Raggatt, P., & Taylor, K. (2005). Foetal testosterone, social relationships, and restricted interests in children. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 46(2), 198–210.

Knickmeyer, R., Baron-Cohen, S., Raggatt, P., Taylor, K., & Hackett, G. (2006). Fetal testosterone and empathy. Hormones and Behavior, 49, 282–292.

Knickmeyer, R. C., Wheelwright, S., Taylor, K., Ragatt, P., Hackett, G., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2005). Gender-typed play and amniotic testosterone. Developmental Psychology, 41(3), 517–528.

Koenig, A. M., & Eagly, A. H. (2005). Stereotype threat in men on a test of social sensitivity. Sex Roles, 52(7/8), 489–496.

Kolata, G. (1983). Math genius may have hormonal basis. Science, 222(4630), 1312.

———. (1995, February 28). Man’s world, woman’s world? Brain studies point to differences. New York Times, C1.

Krendl, A. C., Richeson, J. A., Kelley, W. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (2008). The negative consequences of threat: A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of the neural mechanisms underlying women’s underperformance in math. Psychological Science, 19(2), 168–175.

Kriegeskorte, N., Simmons, W. K., Bellgowan, P. S. F., & Baker, C. I. (2009). Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: The dangers of double dipping. Nature Neuroscience, 12(5), 535–540.

Kunda, Z., & Spencer, S. J. (2003). When do stereotypes come to mind and when do they color judgment? A goal-based theoretical framework for stereotype activation and application. Psychological Bulletin, 129(4), 522–544.

Lamb, M. E., Easterbrooks, M. A., & Holden, G. W. (1980). Reinforcement and punishment among preschoolers: Characteristics, effects, and correlates. Child Development, 51(4), 1230–1236.

Lamb, M. E., & Roopnarine, J. L. (1979). Peer influences on sex-role development in preschoolers. Child Development, 50(4), 1219.

Lamb, S., & Brown, L. (2006). Packaging girlhood: Rescuing our daughters from marketers’ schemes. New York: St Martin’s Press.

Lawrence, P. A. (2006). Men, women, and ghosts in science. PLoS Biology, 4(1), 13–15.

Lawson, A. (2007, May 23). The princess gene. The Age, 18.

Leaper, C., Anderson, K. J., & Sanders, P. (1998). Moderators of gender effects on parents’ talk to their children: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 34(1), 3–27.

Leaper, C., Breed, L., Hoffman, L., & Perlman, C. (2002). Variations in the gender-stereotyped content of children’s television cartoons across genres. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32(8), 1653–1662.

Leeb, R. T., & Rejskind, F. G. (2004). Here’s looking at you, kid! A longitudinal study of perceived gender differences in mutual gaze behavior in young infants. Sex Roles, 50(1/2), 1–14.

Lehrer, J. (2008, August 17). Of course I love you, and I have the brain scan to prove it – We’re looking for too much in brain scans. Boston Globe. K1.

Leinbach, M. D., Hort, B. E., & Fagot, B. I. (1997). Bears are for boys: Metaphorical associations in young children’s gender stereotypes. Cognitive Development, 12, 107–130.

Leinbach, M. D., Hort, B., & Fagot, B. I. (1993). Metaphorical dimensions and the gender-typing of toys. Paper presented at the Symposium conducted at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, New Orleans, Louisiana.

Lenroot, R. K., Gogtay, N., Greenstein, D. K., Wells, E. M., Wallace G. L., Clasen, L. S., et al. (2007). Sexual dimorphism of brain developmental trajectories during childhood and adolescence. NeuroImage, 36, 1065–1073.

Leonard, C. M., Towler, S., Welcome, S., Halderman, L. K., Otto, R., Eckert, M. A., & Chiarello, C. (2008). Size matters: Cerebral volume influences sex differences in neuroanatomy. Cerebral Cortex, 18(12), 2920–2931.

Levine, S. C., Vasilyeva, M., Lourenco, S. F., Newcombe, N. S., & Huttenlocher, J. (2005). Socioeconomic status modifies the sex difference in spatial skills. Psychological Science, 16(11), 841–845.

Levy, G. D., & Haaf, R. A. (1994). Detection of gender-related categories by 10-month-old infants. Infant Behavior & Development, 17(4), 457–459.

Levy, N. (2004). Book review: Understanding blindness. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 3, 315–324.

Lewontin, R. (2000). It ain’t necessarily so: The dream of the human genome and other illusions. New York: New York Review of Books.

Liben, L., Bigler, R., & Krogh, H. (2001). Pink and blue collar jobs: Children’s judgments of job status and job aspirations in relation to sex of worker. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79(4), 346–363.

Lickliter, R., & Honeycutt, H. (2003). Developmental dynamics: Toward a biologically plausible evolutionary psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 129(6), 819–835.

Lockwood, P. (2006). ‘Someone like me can be successful’: Do college students need same-gender role models? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 30(1), 36–46.

Lockwood, P., & Kunda, Z. (1997). Superstars and me: Predicting the impact of role models on the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(1), 91–103.

Logel, C., Iserman, E. C., Davies, P. G., Quinn, D. M., & Spencer, S. J. (2008). The perils of double consciousness: The role of thought suppression in stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(2), 299–312.

Logel, C., Walton, G. M., Spencer, S. J., Iserman, E. C., von Hippel, W., & Bell, A. E. (2009). Interacting with sexist men triggers social identity threat among female engineers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(6), 1089–1103.

Luders, E., Steinmetz, H., & Jäncke, L. (2002). Brain size and grey matter volume in the healthy human brain. NeuroReport, 13(17), 2371–2374.

Lutchmaya, S., Baron-Cohen, S., & Ragatt, P. (2002). Foetal testosterone and eye contact in 12-month-old human infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 25, 327–335.

Lynn, M. (2006, January 12). On bankers and lap dancers. International Herald Tribune, 18.

Lytton, H., & Romney, D. M. (1991). Parents’ differential socialization of boys and girls: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 109(2), 267–296.

MacAdam, G. (1914, January 18). Feminist revolutionary principle is biological bosh. New York Times, SM2 (online archive).

Machin, S., & Pekkarinen, T. (2008). Global sex differences in test score variability. Science, 322, 1331–1332.

Maguire, E. A., Gadian, D. G., Johnsrude, I. S., Good, C. D., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., et al. (2000). Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 97(8), 4398–4403.

Malebranche, N. (1997). The search after truth (T. Lennon & P. Olscamp, trans.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Mareschal, D., Johnson, M. H., Sirois, S., Spratling, M. W., Thomas, M. S. C., & Westermann, G. (2007). Neuroconstructivism: How the brain constructs cognition (vol. 1). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Martin, C. L. (1990). Attitudes and expectations about children with nontraditional and traditional gender roles. Sex Roles, 22(3/4), 151–166.

Martin, C. L., Eisenbud, L., & Rose, H. (1995). Children’s gender-based reasoning about toys. Child Development, 66(5), 1453–1471.

Martin, C. L., & Halverson, C. F. (1981). A schematic processing model of sex typing and stereotyping in children. Child Development, 52, 1119–1134.

Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. (2004). Children’s search for gender cues: Cognitive perspectives on gender development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(2), 67–70.

Martin, C.L., Ruble, D. N., & Szkrybalo, J. (2002). Cognitive theories of early gender development. Psychological Bulletin, 128(6), 903–933.

Martin, K. A. (2005). William wants a doll. Can he have one? Feminists, child care advisors, and gender-neutral child rearing. Gender & Society, 19(4), 456–479.

Marton, F., Fensham, P., & Chaiklin, S. (1994). A Nobel’s eye view of scientific intuition: Discussions with the Nobel prize–winners in physics, chemistry and medicine (1970–86). International Journal of Science Education, 16(4), 457–473.

Marx, D. M., Gilbert, P., Monroe, A., & Cole, C. (unpublished manuscript). Superstars close to me: The effect of role model closeness on performance under threat.

Marx, D. M., & Roman, J. S. (2002). Female role models: Protecting women’s math test performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(9), 1183–1193.

Marx, D. M., & Stapel, D. A. (2006a). Distinguishing stereotype threat from priming effects: On the role of the social self and threat-based concerns. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(2), 243–254.

———. (2006b). It’s all in the timing: Measuring emotional reactions to stereotype threat before and after taking a test. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 687–698.

———. (2006c). It depends on your perspective: The role of self-relevance in stereotype-based underperformance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 768–775.

Marx, D. M., Stapel, D. A., & Muller, D. (2005). We can do it: The interplay of construal orientation and social comparisons under threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(3), 432–446.

Mason, M. A., & Goulden, M. (2004). Marriage and baby blues: Redefining gender equity in the academy. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 596(1), 86–103.

Mason, W. A. (2002). The natural history of primate behavioral development: An organismic perspective. In D. J. Lewkowicz & R. Lickliter (eds.), Conceptions of development: Lessons from the laboratory (pp. 105–134). New York, London, and Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

Mast, M. S. (2004). Men are hierarchical, women are egalitarian: An implicit gender stereotype. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 63(2), 107–111.

Masters, J. C., Ford, M. E., Arend, R., Grotevant, H. D., & Clark, L. V. (1979). Modeling and labeling as integrated determinants of children’s sex-typed imitative behavior. Child Development, 50, 364–371.

Mathews, G. A., Fane, B. A., Conway, G. S., Brook, C.G.D., & Hines, M. (2009). Personality and congenital adrenal hyperplasia: Possible effects of prenatal androgen exposure. Hormones and Behavior, 55, 285–291.

Mathews, G. A., Fane, B. A., Pasterski, V. L., Conway, G. S., Brook, C., & Hines, M. (2004). Androgenic influences on neural asymmetry: Handedness and language lateralization in individuals with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29(6), 810–822.

McCabe, D. P., & Castel, A. D. (2008). Seeing is believing: The effect of brain images on judgments of scientific reasoning. Cognition, 107, 343–352.

McClure, E. B. (2000). A meta-analytic review of sex differences in facial expression processing and their development in infants, children, and adolescents. Psychological Bulletin, 126(3), 424–453.

McCrum, R. (2008, August 24). Up Pompeii with the roguish don. The Observer, 22.

McGlone, M. S., & Aronson, J. (2006). Stereotype threat, identity salience, and spatial reasoning. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27(5), 486–493.

McIntyre, M. H. (2006). The use of digit ratios as markers for perinatal androgen action. Reproductive Biology and Endocrinology, 4(1), 10.

McIntyre, R. B., Lord, C. G., Gresky, D. M., Ten Eyck, L. L., Frye, G. D. J., & Bond, C. F. (2005). A social impact trend in the effects of role models on alleviating women’s mathematics stereotype threat. Current Research in Social Psychology, 10(9), 116–136.

McIntyre, R. B., Paulson, R. M., & Lord, C. G. (2003). Alleviating women’s mathematics stereotype threat through salience of group achievements. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 83–90.

Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F., Dolezal, C., Baker, S. W., Carlson, A. D., Obeid, J. S., & New, M. I. (2004). Prenatal androgenization affects gender-related behavior but not gender identity in 5–12-year-old girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 33(2), 97–104.

Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F., Dolezal, C., Zucker, K. J., Kessler, S. J., Schober, J. M., & New, M. I. (2006). The recalled Childhood Gender Questionnaire–revised: A psychometric analysis in a sample of women with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Journal of Sex Research, 43(4), 364–367.

Mill, J. (1869/1988). The subjection of women. Indianapolis: Hackett Publishing Company.

Miller, C. F., Lurye, L. E., Zosuls, K. M., & Ruble, D. N. (2009). Accessibility of gender stereotype domains: Developmental and gender differences in children. Sex Roles, 60(11/12), 870–881.

Miller, C. F., Trautner, H. M., & Ruble, D. N. (2006). The role of gender stereotypes in children’s preferences and behavior. In L. Balter & C. Tamis-LeMonda (eds.), Child psychology: A handbook of contemporary issues (2nd ed., pp. 293–323). New York: Psychology Press.

Miller, G. (2008). Growing pains for fMRI. Science, 320 (5882), 1412–1414.

Moè, A. (2009). Are males always better than females in mental rotation? Exploring a gender belief explanation. Learning and Individual Differences, 19(1), 21–27.

Moè, A., & Pazzaglia, F. (2006). Following the instructions! Effects of gender beliefs in mental rotation. Learning and Individual Differences, 16(4), 369–377.

Moir, A., & Jessel, D. (1989). Brain sex: The real difference between men and women. London: Michael Joseph.

Monastersky, R. (2005, March 4). Primed for numbers. (Lawrence H. Summers). Chronicle of Higher Education, 51(26): NA.

Mondschein, E. R., Adolph, K. E., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2000). Gender bias in mothers’ expectations about infant crawling. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77(4), 304–316.

Montemurri, P. (2009, April 9). Gloria Steinem: Women’s liberation is ‘longest revolution’. From Star-Telegram.com: http://www.star-telegram.com/living/story/1309400.html. Accessed on November 4, 2009.

Moon, C., Cooper, R. P., & Fifer, W. P. (1993). Two-day-olds prefer their native language. Infant Behavior and Development, 16(4), 495–500.

Moore, C. L. (1995). Maternal contributions to mammalian reproductive development and the divergence of males and females. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 24, 47–118.

Moore, C. L. (2002). On differences and development. In D. J. Lewkowicz & R. Lickliter (eds.), Conceptions of development: Lessons from the laboratory (pp. 57–76). New York: Psychology Press.

Moore, C. L., Dou, H., & Juraska, J. M. (1992). Maternal stimulation affects the number of motor neurons in a sexually dimorphic nucleus of the lumbar spinal cord. Brain Research, 572(1/2), 52–56.

Moore, D. S., & Johnson, S. P. (2008). Mental rotation in human infants: A sex difference. Psychological Science, 19(11), 1063–1066.

Morantz-Sanchez, R. M. (1985). Sympathy and science: Women physicians in American medicine. New York and Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Morgan, L. A., & Martin, K. A. (2006). Taking women professionals out of the office: The case of women in sales. Gender and Society, 20(1), 108–128.

Morris, J. (1987). Conundrum. Harmondsworth, Middlesex: Penguin Books.

Morris, J. A., Jordan, C. L., & Breedlove, S. M. (2004). Sexual differentiation of the vertebrate nervous system. Nature Neuroscience, 7(10), 1034–1039.

Morton, T. A., Haslam, S. A., Postmes, T., & Ryan, M. K. (2006). We value what values us: The appeal of identity-affirming science. Political Psychology, 27(6), 823–838.

Morton, T. A., Postmes, T., Haslam, S. A., & Hornsey, M. J. (2009). Theorizing gender in the face of social change: Is there anything essential about essentialism? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(3), 653–664.

Mullarkey, M. J. (2004). Two Harvard women: 1965 to today. Harvard Journal of Law & Gender, 27(Spring), 367–379.

Murphy, M. C., Steele, C. M., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Signaling threat: How situational cues affect women in math, science, and engineering settings. Psychological Science, 18(10), 879–885.

Mussweiler, T., Rüter, K., & Epstude, K. (2004). The ups and downs of social comparison: Mechanisms of assimilation and contrast. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(6), 832–844.

Nash, A., & Grossi, G. (2007). Picking Barbie’s brain: Inherent sex differences in scientific ability? Journal of Interdisciplinary Feminist Thought, 2(1), Article 5.

Nash, A., & Krawczyk, R. (1994). Boys’ and girls’ rooms revisited: The contents of boys’ and girls’ rooms in the 1990s. Paper presented at the Conference on Human Development, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania.

Neuville, E., & Croizet, J.-C. (2007). Can salience of gender identity impair math performance among 7-8 years old girls? The moderating role of task difficulty. European Journal of Psychology of Education, XXII(3), 307–316.

Newcombe, N. S. (2007). Taking science seriously: Straight thinking about spatial sex differences. In S. Ceci & W. Williams (eds.), Why aren’t more women in science? Top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 69–77). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Newman, M. L., Sellers, J. G., & Josephs, R. A. (2005). Testosterone, cognition, and social status. Hormones and Behavior, 47(2), 205–211.

Nguyen, H. H., & Ryan, A. M. (2008). Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(6), 1314–1334.

Nordenström, A., Servin, A., Bohlin, G., Larsson, A., & Wedell, A. (2002). Sex-typed toy play behavior correlates with the degree of prenatal androgen exposure assessed by CYP21 genotype in girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 87(11), 5119–5124.

Norton, M. I., Vandello, J. A., & Darley, J. M. (2004). Casuistry and social category bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(6), 817–831.

Nosek, B. A. (2007a). Implicit-explicit relations. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(2), 65–69.

———. (2007b). Understanding the individual implicitly and explicitly. International Journal of Psychology, 42(3), 184–188.

Nosek, B. A., & Hansen, J. (2008). The associations in our heads belong to us: Searching for attitudes and knowledge in implicit evaluation. Cognition and Emotion, 22(4), 553–594.

Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Hansen, J. J., Devos, T., Lindner, N. M., Ranganath, K. A., et al. (2007). Pervasiveness and correlates of implicit attitudes and stereotypes. European Review of Social Psychology, 18(1), 36–88.

Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Sriram, N., Lindner, N. M., Devos, T., Ayala, A., et al. (2009). National differences in gender-science stereotypes predict national sex differences in science and math achievement. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(26), 10593–10597.

Novell, C. (2004). Disney princess: How to be a princess. Camberwell, Victoria: Penguin Books.

Nowlan, M. (2006, October 2). Women doctors, their ranks growing, transform medicine. Boston Globe, C1.

O’Boyle, M. W. (2005). Some current findings on brain characteristics of the mathematically gifted adolescent. International Education Journal, 6(2), 247–251.

O’Boyle, M. W., Cunnington, R., Silk, T. J., Vaughn, D., Jackson, G., Syngeniotis, A., & Egan, G. F. (2005). Mathematically gifted male adolescents activate a unique brain network during mental rotation. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(2), 583–587.

Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Altschuler, E. L., Ramachandran, V. S., & Pineda, J. A. (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 190–198.

Ohnishi, T., Moriguchi, Y., Matsuda, H., Mori, T., Hirakata, M., Imabayashi, E., et al. (2004). The neural network for the mirror system and mentalizing in normally developed children: An fMRI study. NeuroReport, 15(9), 1483–1487.

Onorato, R. S., & Turner, J. C. (2004). Fluidity in the self-concept: The shift from personal to social identity. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 257–278.

Orenstein, P. (2000). Flux: Women on sex, work, love, kids, and life in a half-changed world. New York: Anchor Books.

Orzhekhovskaia, N. S. (2005). [Sex dimorphism of neuron-glia correlations in the frontal areas of the human brain]. Morfologiia, 127(1), 7–9.

Paechter, C. (2007). Being boys, Being girls: Learning masculinities and femininities. Maidenhead, Berkshire: Open University Press.

Paoletti, J. B. (1997). The gendering of infants’ and toddlers’ clothing in America. In K. Martinez & K. L. Ames (eds.), The material culture of gender: The gender of material culture (pp. 27–35). Hanover, NH, and London: University Press of New England.

Pasterski, V. L., Geffner, M. E., Brain, C., Hindmarsh, P., Brook, C., & Hines, M. (2005). Prenatal hormones and postnatal socialization by parents as determinants of male-typical toy play in girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Child Development, 76(1), 264–278.

Patterson, M. M., & Bigler, R. S. (2006). Preschool children’s attention to environmental messages about groups: Social categorization and the origins of intergroup bias. Child Development, 77(4), 847–860.

Pease, A., & Pease, B. (2008). Why men don’t listen and women can’t read maps. Pease International Pty Ltd.

Penner, A. (2008). Gender differences in extreme mathematical achievement: An international perspective on biological and social factors. American Journal of Sociology, 114, S138–170.

Peplau, L. A., & Fingerhut, A. (2004). The paradox of the lesbian worker. Journal of Social Issues, 60(4), 719–735.

Phelan, J. E., Moss-Racusin, C. A., & Rudman, L. A. (2008). Competent yet out in the cold: Shifting criteria for hiring reflect backlash toward agentic women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32, 406–413.

Pierce, A. (2009, February 23). The epitome of a ‘very pretty’ Tory lady; Unseen papers show how a young Thatcher charmed BBC executives. Daily Telegraph, 5.

Pike, J. J., & Jennings, N. A. (2005). The effects of commercials on children’s perceptions of gender appropriate toy use. Sex Roles, 52(1/2), 83–91.

Pinker, S. (2005, February 14). Sex Ed. From the New Republic: http://www.tnr.com/article/sex-ed. Accessed November 18, 2009.

Pinker, S. (2008). The sexual paradox: Men, women, and the real gender gap. New York: Scribner.

Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(2), 59–63.

Poldrack, R. A., & Wagner, A. D. (2004). What can neuroimaging tell us about the mind? Insights from prefrontal cortex. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(5), 177–181.

Pomerleau, A., Bolduc, D., Malcuit, G., & Cossette, L. (1990). Pink or blue: Environmental gender stereotypes in the first two years of life. Sex Roles, 22(5/6), 359–367.

Poulin-Dubois, D., Serbin, L. A., Eichstedt, J. A., Sen, M. G., & Beissel, C. F. (2002). Men don’t put on make-up: Toddlers’ knowledge of the gender stereotyping of household activities. Social Development, 11(2), 166–181.

Prime, J., Jonsen, K., Carter, N., & Maznevski, M. L. (2008). Managers’ perceptions of women and men leaders: A cross cultural comparison. International Journal of Cross Cultural Management, 8(2), 171–210.

Pronin, E., Steele, C. M., & Ross, L. (2004). Identity bifurcation in response to stereotype threat: Women and mathematics. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 152–168.

Puts, D. A., McDaniel, M. A., Jordan, C. L., & Breedlove, S. M. (2008). Spatial ability and prenatal androgens: Meta-analyses of congenital adrenal hyperplasia and digit ratio (2D:4D) studies. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37(1), 100–111.

Quinn, P. C., & Liben, L. S. (2008). A sex difference in mental rotation in young infants. Psychological Science, 19(11), 1067–1070.

Quinn, P. C., Yahr, J., Kuhn, A., Slater, A. M., & Pascalis, O. (2002). Representation of the gender of human faces by infants: A preference for female. Perception, 31(9), 1109–1121.

Racine, E., Bar-Ilan, O., & Illes, J. (2005). fMRI in the public eye. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(2), 159–164.

Raingruber, B. J. (2001). Settling into and moving in a climate of care: Styles and patterns of interaction between nurse psychotherapists and clients. Perspectives in Psychiatric Care, 37(1), 15–27.

Realo, A., Allik, J., Nõlvak, A., Valk, R., Ruus, T., Schmidt, M., et al. (2003). Mind-reading ability: Beliefs and performance. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 420–445.

Rhode, D. L. (1997). Speaking of sex: The denial of gender inequality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Ridgeway, C. L., & Correll, S. J. (2004). Unpacking the gender system: A theoretical perspective on gender beliefs and social relations. Gender & Society, 18(4), 510–531.

Rivers, C., & Barnett, R. C. (2007, October 28). The difference myth. Boston Globe, F1.

Rochat, P. (2001). The infant’s world. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Rogers, K., Dziobek, I., Hassenstab, K., Wolf, O., & Convit, A. (2007). Who cares? Revisiting empathy in Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(4), 709–715.

Rogers, L. (1999). Sexing the brain. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.

Rogers, L. J., Zucca, P., & Vallortigara, G. (2004). Advantages of having a lateralized brain. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Ser. B, 271, S420–S422.

Romanes, G. J. (1887/1987). Mental differences between men and women. In D. Spender (ed.), Education papers: Women’s quest for equality in Britain, 1850–1912. London and New York: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

Rosenblatt, J. S. (1967). Nonhormonal basis of maternal behavior in the rat. Science, 156(3781), 1512–1513.

Roth, L. M. (2004). Bringing clients back in: Homophily preferences and inequality on Wall Street. Sociological Quarterly, 45(4), 613–635.

Rothman, B. (1988). The tentative pregnancy: Prenatal diagnosis and the future of motherhood. London: Pandora.

Ruble, D., Lurye, L., & Zosuls, K. (2008). Pink frilly dresses (PFD) and early gender identity [Electronic Version]. Princeton Report on Knowledge. http://www.princeton.edu/prok/issues/2-2/pink_frilly.xml. Accessed on April 23, 2008.

Rudman, L. A. (1998). Self-promotion as a risk factor for women: The costs and benefits of counterstereotypical impression management. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(3), 629–645.

Rudman, L. A., & Glick, P. (1999). Feminized management and backlash toward agentic women: The hidden costs to women of a kinder, gentler image of middle managers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(5), 1004–1010.

———. (2001). Prescriptive gender stereotypes and backlash toward agentic women. Journal of Social Issues, 57(4), 743–762.

———. (2008). The social psychology of gender: How power and intimacy shape gender relations. New York: The Guilford Press.

Rudman, L. A., & Heppen, J. B. (2003). Implicit romantic fantasies and women’s interest in personal power: A glass slipper effect? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(11), 1357–1370.

Rudman, L. A., & Kilianski, S. E. (2000). Implicit and explicit attitudes toward female authority. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(11), 1315–1328.

Rudman, L. A., Phelan, J. E., & Heppen, J. B. (2007). Developmental sources of implicit attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(12), 1700–1713.

Rudman, L. A., Phelan, J. E., Moss-Rascusin, C. A., & Nauts, S. (manuscript submitted for publication). Status incongruity and backlash effects: Defending the gender hierarchy motivates prejudice toward female leaders.

Rush, E., & La Nauze, A. (2006). Corporate paedophilia: Sexualisation of children in Australia. The Australia Institute, Canberra.

Russett, C. E. (1989). Sexual science: The Victorian construction of womanhood. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Ryan, M. K., David, B., & Reynolds, K. J. (2004). Who cares? The effect of gender and context on the self and moral reasoning. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 28, 246–255.

Ryan, M. K., Haslam, S. A., Hersby, M. D., Kulich, C., & Atkins, C. (2007). Opting out or pushed off the edge? The glass cliff and the precariousness of women’s leadership positions. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 1(1), 266–279.

Sandnabba, N. K., & Ahlberg, C. (1999). Parents’ attitudes and expectations about children’s cross-gender behavior. Sex Roles, 40(3/4), 249–263.

Sani, F., Bennett, M., Mullally, S., & MacPherson, J. (2003). On the assumption of fixity in children’s stereotypes: A reappraisal. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 113–124.

Sapolsky, R. (1997, October). A gene for nothing. From Discover Magazine online: http://discovermagazine.com/1997/oct/agenefornothing1242. Accessed on December 12, 2009.

Sax, L. (2005, January 23). Too few women – figure it out. Los Angeles Times, M5.

———. (2006). Why gender matters: What parents and teachers need to know about the emerging science of sex differences. New York: Broadway Books.

Schaffer, A. (2008, July 1). The sex difference evangelists. From Slate: http://www.slate.com/id/2194486/entry/2194487. Accessed on June 11, 2009.

Schilt, K. (2006). Just one of the guys? How transmen make gender visible at work. Gender and Society, 20(4), 465–490.

Schmader, T., & Johns, M. (2003). Converging evidence that stereotype threat reduces working memory capacity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(3), 440–452.

Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Barquissau, M. (2004). The costs of accepting gender differences: The role of stereotype endorsement in women’s experience in the math domain. Sex Roles, 50(11/12), 835–850.

Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Forbes, C. (2008). An integrated process model of stereotype threat effects on performance. Psychological Review, 115(2), 336–356.

Schneider, F., Habel, U., Kessler, C., Salloum, J. B., & Posse, S. (2000). Gender differences in regional cerebral activity during sadness. Human Brain Mapping, 9, 226–238.

Schweder, R. S., & Sullivan, M. A. (1993). Cultural psychology: Who needs it? Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 497–523.

Seger, C. R., Smith, E. R., & Mackie, D. M. (2009). Subtle activation of a social categorization triggers group-level emotions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(3), 460–467.

Seibt, B., & Förster, J. (2004). Stereotype threat and performance: How self-stereotypes influence processing by inducing regulatory foci. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(1), 38–56.

Selmi, M. (2005). Sex discrimination in the nineties, seventies style: Case studies in the preservation of male workplace norms. Employee Rights and Employment Policy Journal, 9, 1–50.

———. (2007). The work-family conflict: An essay on employers, men and responsibility. University of St. Thomas Law Journal, 4, 573–598.

Senior, A. (2009, May 29). The pernicious pinkification of little girls. The Times [London], 31.

Serbin, L. A., Connor, J. M., Burchardt, C. J., & Citron, C. C. (1979). Effects of peer presence on sex-typing of children’s play behavior. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 27, 303–309.

Serbin, L. A., Poulin-Dubois, D., & Eichstedt, J. A. (2002). Infants’ responses to gender-inconsistent events. Infancy, 3(4), 531–542.

Servin, A., Bohlin, G., & Berlin, L. (1999). Sex differences in 1-, 3-, and 5-year-olds’ toy-choice in a structured play-session. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 40(1), 43–48.

Servin, A., Bohlin, G., Nordenstrom, A., & Larsson, A. (2003). Prenatal androgens and gender-typed behavior: A study of girls with mild and severe forms of congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Developmental Psychology, 39(3), 440–450.

Shah, J. (2003). Automatic for the people: How representations of significant others implicitly affect goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 661–681.

Shapiro, J. R., & Neuberg, S. L. (2007). From stereotype threat to stereotype threats: Implications of a multi-threat framework for causes, moderators, mediators, consequences, and interventions. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11(2), 107–130.

Sharps, M. J., Price, J. L., & Williams, J. K. (1994). Spatial cognition and gender: Instructional and stimulus influences on mental image rotation performance. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18(3), 413–425.

Sheldon, J. (2004). Gender stereotypes in educational software for young children. Sex Roles, 51(7/8), 433–444.

Sherwin, B. B. (1988). A comparative analysis of the role of androgens in human male and female sexual behavior: Behavioral specificity, critical thresholds, and sensitivity. Psychobiology, 16(4), 416–425.

Shields, S. (1975). Functionalism, Darwinism, and the psychology of women: A study in social myth. American Psychologist, 30(7), 739–754.

———. (1982). The variability hypothesis: The history of a biological model of sex differences in intelligence. SIGNS: Journal of Women in Culture & Society, 7(4), 769–797.

Silverberg, A. (2006). Remarks at an Association for Women in Mathematics panel [entitled ‘Lawrence Summers: One year later’]. Presented at the Joint Mathematics Meetings, San Antonio, Texas.

Silvers, J. A., & Haidt, J. (2008). Moral elevation can induce nursing. Emotion, 8(2), 291–295.

Sinclair, L., & Kunda, Z. (2000). Motivated stereotyping of women: She’s fine if she praised me but incompetent if she criticized me. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(11), 1329–1342.

Sinclair, S., Hardin, C. D., & Lowery, B. S. (2006). Self-stereotyping in the context of multiple social identities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(4), 529–542.

Sinclair, S., Huntsinger, J., Skorinko, J., & Hardin, C. D. (2005). Social tuning of the self: Consequences for the self-evaluations of stereotype targets. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(2), 160–175.

Sinclair, S., & Lun, J. (2006). Significant other representations activate stereotypic self-views among women. Self and Identity, 5, 196–207.

Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science, 303(1157), 1157–1162.

Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J. P., Stephan, K. E., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature, 439(7075), 466–469.

Singh, H., & O’Boyle, M. W. (2004). Interhemispheric interaction during global-local processing in mathematically gifted adolescents, average-ability youth, and college students. Neuropsychology, 18(2), 371–377.

Skuse, D. H. (2009). Is autism really a coherent syndrome in boys, or girls? British Journal of Psychology, 100, 33–37.

Smith, E. R., & DeCoster, J. (2000). Dual-process models in social and cognitive psychology: Conceptual integration and links to underlying memory systems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4(2), 108–131.

Smith, J. (1998). Different for girls: How culture creates women. London: Vintage.

Smith, K. (2005). Prebirth gender talk: A case study in gender socialization. Women and Language, 28(1), 49–54.

Smith, S., & Cook, C. (2008). Gender stereotypes: An analysis of popular films and TV. Geena Davis Institute on Gender in Media: http://www.thegeenadavisinstitute.org.

Sommer, I. E. C., Aleman, A., Bouma, A., & Kahn, R. S. (2004). Do women really have more bilateral language representation than men? A meta-analysis of functional imaging studies. Brain, 127, 1845–1852.

Sommer, I. E. C., Aleman, A., Somers, M., Boks, M. P., & Kahn, R. S. (2008). Sex differences in handedness, asymmetry of the Planum Temporale and functional language lateralization. Brain Research, 1206, 76–88.

Spelke, E. S. (2005). Sex differences in intrinsic aptitude for mathematics and science? A critical review. American Psychologist, 60(9), 950–958.

Spelke, E. S., & Grace, A. D. (2006). Abilities, motives, and personal styles. American Psychologist, 61(7), 725–726.

Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and women’s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35(1), 4–28.

Stangor, C., Carr, C., & Kiang, L. (1998). Activating stereotypes undermines task performance expectations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(5), 1191–1197.

Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52(6), 613–629.

Steele, J. R., & Ambady, N. (2006). ‘Math is hard!’ The effect of gender priming on women’s attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(4), 428–436.

Steinpreis, R. E., Anders, K. A., & Ritzke, D. (1999). The impact of gender on the review of the curricula vitae of job applicants and tenure candidates: A national empirical study. Sex Roles, 41(7/8), 509–528.

Stone, P. (2007). Opting out? Why women really quit careers and head home. Berkeley: University of California Press.

Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(3), 220–247.

Summers, L. (2005). Remarks at National Bureau of Economics Research conference on diversifying the science & engineering workforce, Cambridge, Massachusetts. Available at http://www.president.harvard.edu/speeches/summers_2005/nber.php. Accessed January 7, 2009.

Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In S. Worchel & W. Austin (eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 7–24). Chicago: Nelson-Hall Publishers.

Tavris, C. (1992). The mismeasure of woman: Why women are not the better sex, the inferior sex, or the opposite sex. New York: Touchstone.

Telford, L. (2003). My pretty princess beauty kit and book. New York: Disney Press.

Tenenbaum, H. R., & Leaper, C. (2002). Are parents’ gender schemas related to their children’s gender-related cognitions? A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 38(4), 615–630.

Tepper, C. A., & Cassidy, K. W. (1999). Gender differences in emotional language in children’s picture books. Sex Roles, 40(3/4), 265–280.

Thoman, D. B., White, P. H., Yamawaki, N., & Koishi, H. (2008). Variations of gender-math stereotype content affect women’s vulnerability to stereotype threat. Sex Roles, 58, 702–712.

Thomas, G., & Maio, G. R. (2008). Man, I feel like a woman: When and how gender-role motivation helps mind-reading. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(5), 1165–1179.

Thompson, S. K. (1975). Gender labels and early sex role development. Child Development, 46, 339–347.

Thompson, T. L., & Zerbinos, E. (1995). Gender roles in animated cartoons: Has the picture changed in 20 years? Sex Roles, 32(9/10), 651–673.

Tichenor, V. (2005). Maintaining men’s dominance: Negotiating identity and power when she earns more. Sex Roles, 53(3/4), 191–205.

Trautner, H. M., Ruble, D. N., Cyphers, L., Kirsten, B., Behrendt, R., & Hartmann, P. (2005). Rigidity and flexibility of gender stereotypes in childhood: Developmental or differential? Infant and Child Development, 14(4), 365–381.

Trecker, J. (1974). Sex, science and education. American Quarterly, 26(4), 352–366.

Turner-Bowker, D. M. (1996). Gender stereotyped descriptors in children’s picture books: Does ‘Curious Jane’ exist in the literature? Sex Roles, 35(7/8), 461–488.

Uddin, L. Q., Kaplan, J. T., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Zaidel, E., & Iacoboni, M. (2005). Self-face recognition activates a frontoparietal ‘mirror’ network in the right hemisphere: An event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage, 25, 926–935.

Udry, J. R. (2000). Biological limits of gender construction. American Sociological Review, 65, 443–457.

Uhlmann, E. L., & Cohen, G. L. (2005). Constructed criteria. Psychological Science, 16(6), 474–480.

University of Pennsylvania Medical Center (1999). Sex differences found in proportions of gray and white matter in the brain. http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/1999/05/990518072823.htm. Accessed November 6, 2008.

Unterrainer, J., Wranek, U., Staffen, W., Gruber, T., & Ladurner, G. (2000). Lateralized cognitive visuospatial processing: Is it primarily gender-related or due to quality of performance? Neuropsychobiology, 41(2), 95–101.

Valian, V. (1998). Why so slow? The advancement of women. Cambridge, MA, and London: MIT Press.

van Anders, S. M., & Watson, N. V. (2006). Social neuroendocrinology: Effects of social contexts and behaviors on sex steroids in humans. Human Nature, 17(2), 212–237.

van de Beek, C., Thijssen, J. H. H., Cohen-Kettenis, P. T., van Goozen, S. H. M., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2004). Relationships between sex hormones assessed in amniotic fluid, and maternal and umbilical cord serum: What is the best source of information to investigate the effects of fetal hormonal exposure? Hormones and Behavior, 46(5), 663–669.

van de Beek, C., van Goozen, S. H. M., Buitelaar, J. K., & Cohen-Kettenis, P. T. (2009). Prenatal sex hormones (maternal and amniotic fluid) and gender-related play behavior in 13-month-old infants. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38, 6–15.

Verghis, S. (2009, May 2). Triumph of the few. Good Weekend, 21–26.

Vespa, J. (2009). Gender ideology construction: A life course and intersectional approach. Gender and Society, 23(3), 363–387.

Voracek, M., & Dressler, S. G. (2006). Lack of correlation between digit ratio (2D:4D) and Baron-Cohen’s ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’ test, empathy, systemising, and autism-spectrum quotients in a general population sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 1481–1491.

Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117(2), 250–270.

Vul, E., Harris, C., Winkelman, P., & Pashler, H. (2009). Puzzlingly high correlations in fMRI studies of emotion, personality, and social cognition. Perspective on Psychological Science, 4(3), 274–290.

Wager, T. D., Phan, K. L., Liberzon, I., & Taylor, S. F. (2003). Valence, gender, and lateralization of functional brain anatomy in emotion: A meta-analysis of findings from neuroimaging. NeuroImage, 19(3), 513–531.

Walker, K. (2008, August 13). All pink and sparkly [Letter to the Editor]. The Age, 12.

Wallen, K. (1996). Nature needs nurture: The interaction of hormonal and social influences on the development of behavioral sex differences in rhesus monkeys. Hormones and Behavior, 30(4), 364–378.

———. (2005). Hormonal influences on sexually differentiated behavior in nonhuman primates. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 26, 7–26.

Wallentin, M. (2009). Putative sex differences in verbal abilities and language cortex: A critical review. Brain and Language, 108(3), 175–183.

Walton, G. M., & Spencer, S. J. (2009). Latent ability: grades and test scores systematically underestimate the intellectual ability of negatively stereotyped students. Psychological Science, 20(9): 1132–1139.

Weichselbaumer, D., & Winter-Ebmer, R. (2005). A meta-analysis of the international gender wage gap. Journal of Economic Surveys, 19(3), 479–511.

Weil, E. (2008, March 2). Teaching to the testosterone. New York Times Magazine, 38.

Weisberg, D. S. (2008). Caveat lector: The presentation of neuroscience information in the popular media. Science Review of Mental Health Practice, 6(1), 51–56.

Weisberg, D. S., Keil, F. C., Goodstein, J., Rawson, E., & Gray, J. R. (2008). The seductive allure of neuroscience explanations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(3), 470–477.

Weitzman, L. J., Eifler, D., Hokada, E., & Ross, C. (1972). Sex-role socialization in picture books for preschool children. American Journal of Sociology, 77(6), 1125–1150.

Weitzman, N., Birns, B., & Friend, R. (1985). Traditional and nontraditional mothers’ communication with their daughters and sons. Child Development, 56(4), 894–898.

Welnsteln, N., Przybylski, A., & Ryan, R. (2009). Can nature make us more caring? Effects of immersion in nature on intrinsic aspirations and generosity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(10), 1315–1329.

Westermann, G., Mareschal, D., Johnson, M. H., Sirois, S., Spratling, M. W., & Thomas, M. S. C. (2007). Neuroconstructivism. Developmental Science, 10(1), 75–83.

Wexler, B. (2006). Brain and culture: Neurobiology, ideology, and social change. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Wheeler, S., DeMarree, K., & Petty, R. E. (2007). Understanding the role of the self in prime-to-behavior effects: The active-self account. Personality and Social Psychology Review 11(3), 234–261.

Williams, C. L. (1992). The glass escalator: Hidden advantages for men in the ‘female’ professions. Social Problems, 39(3), 253–266.

Wingfield, A. (2009). Racializing the glass escalator: Reconsidering men’s experiences with women’s work. Gender and Society, 23(1), 5–26.

Woodzicka, J. A., & LaFrance, M. (2001). Real versus imagined gender harassment. Journal of Social Issues, 57(1), 15–30.

Wraga, M., Helt, M., Jacobs, E., & Sullivan, K. (2006). Neural basis of stereotype-induced shifts in women’s mental rotation performance. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 12–19.

Wynne-Edwards, K. E. (2001). Hormonal changes in mammalian fathers. Hormones and Behavior, 40(2), 139–145.

Wynne-Edwards, K. E., & Reburn, C. J. (2000). Behavioral endocrinology of mammalian fatherhood. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 15(11), 464–468.

Young, R. M., & Balaban, E. (2006). Psychoneuroindoctrinology. Nature, 443, 634.

Zahn-Waxler, C., Klimes-Dougan, B., & Slattery, M. J. (2000). Internalizing problems of childhood and adolescence: Prospects, pitfalls, and progress in understanding the development of anxiety and depression. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 443–466.

Zosuls, K. M., Ruble, D. N., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Shrout, P. E., Bornstein, M. H., & Greulich, F. K. (2009). The acquisition of gender labels in infancy: Implications for sex-typed play. Developmental Psychology, 45(3), 688–701.


Дата добавления: 2019-02-22; просмотров: 171; Мы поможем в написании вашей работы!

Поделиться с друзьями:






Мы поможем в написании ваших работ!